Chapter 22
Visual Dysfunction from Lesions of the Cerebral Cortex
JASON J. S. BARTON and MATTHEW RIZZO
Main Menu   Table Of Contents

Search

LESIONS OF STRIATE CORTEX
LESIONS OF EXTRASTRIATE CORTEX
POSITIVE PHENOMENA
FUNCTIONAL IMAGING AND OTHER TECHNIQUES FOR STUDYING VISION AND CORTEX
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES

The functional segregation of visual inputs in the primate visual system is well established. Retinal information is communicated to cerebral cortical neurons through a set of pathways that appear specialized to convey particular classes of visual information. The parvocellular pathway, named for its relay to simian striate cortex (area V1) via parvocellular layers 3 to 6 of the lateral geniculate body, is characterized by color opponency and slow conducting axons that convey sustained signals.1,2 In contrast, the magnocellular pathway is characterized by large, fast-conducting axons conveying information about transient signals. These pathways terminate in separate layers of striate cortex. In addition to this subcortical segregation of visual processes, processing of vision in extrastriate cortex also appears to involve two dominant parallel but interconnecting pathways. Areas such as V4 and the inferotemporal (IT) region, located in the inferior occipital lobe and adjacent occipitotemporal regions,3 comprise a “ventral” or temporal cortical pathway (the “what” pathway), and are presumed to play a role in the perception of color, luminance, static stereopsis, and pattern recognition. Regions such as the middle temporal (MT) and medial superior temporal (MST) areas comprise the “dorsal” or parietal cortical pathway (the “where” pathway) and are thought to play a role in motion perception and other visuospatial functions. The degree to which the M and P subcortical pathways correspond to the dorsal and ventral cortical pathways, respectively, is a matter of debate4: currently, it appears that there is considerable mixing of M and P input in V2 and beyond, although M input may dominate in the dorsal pathway.

Although the processing of information in the visual pathways relies on multiple channels that interact extensively with each other,4–6 the notion of separate “what” and “where” pathways (i.e., two visual systems) has proved useful in the monkey model.7 Furthermore it provides a convenient framework for interpreting the visual disturbances due to human brain lesions.8 The “what” pathway in humans extends from below the calcarine fissure into the visual association cortex in the adjacent medial temporal lobe, which is thought to contain a human homologue of simian areas V4 and IT. Damage to this pathway is associated with impairments in recognition of objects (the agnosias), reading (the alexias), and color perception (cerebral achromatopsia). In humans, the “where” pathway extends from the dorsal bank of the calcarine fissure into visual association cortices over the superior and lateral surfaces of the posterior hemisphere, which is thought to contain a human homologue of the monkey's area MT complex and parieto-occipital areas. Damage to this pathway can alter visuospatial processing, including the control of visually guided eye and hand movements (as in “Balint's syndrome” ), and can impair motion perception (cerebral akinetopsia). We review these deficits below, which differ from the topographic defects caused by lesions of striate cortex. The question of residual vision in a “destriated” field (i.e., “blindsight”) is also addressed.

Figure 1 shows a map of the visual areas and connections in the monkey's brain.9–15

Fig. 1. Visual cortex of the monkey. The monkey's cortex is depicted as unfolded and splayed flat. Area V1 (Brodmann's area 17) is the main recipient of parallel subcortical channels and a bottleneck for ascending inputs to maps of the visual fields located in extrastriate cortex. Area V1 connects reciprocally with V2, V3, and surrounding regions in early visual association cortex. It also connects directly and reciprocally with areas V5 (MT) and at least part of area V4. Area V4 receives a balance of M and P inputs; projects ventrally toward inferotemporal cortex (area IT); contains neurons modulated by attention, relevance, and perceptual context5,6; and may contribute to color and pattern processing.7,8 In contrast, V5 (MT) receives a predominance of M inputs, and projects dorsally toward the parieto-occipital regions. The neural complex that includes area MT and surrounding regions9,10 is probably important for the processing of motion, attention, and related visuospatial processes.11 Human vision and its disorders can be explained in terms of a homologous organization. (Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC: Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1:1, 1991)

Back to Top
LESIONS OF STRIATE CORTEX
Brodmann16 used cytoarchitectonic criteria to divide the human cortex into several different areas. He designated the striate cortex or primary visual cortex as area 17 (Fig. 2). He showed that this area, now also known as area V1, surrounds the calcarine fissure symmetrically, which is why V1 is also sometimes referred to as the calcarine cortex. Another name for V1, striate cortex, refers to a unique histologic feature: the line (stria) of Gennari, a thin but easily visible white lamina that runs parallel to the cortical surface within the gray matter. The exact position of V1 can vary somewhat among individuals, although the parieto-occipital fissure forms a fairly reliable anterior dorsal boundary. Posteriorly, the foveal representation extends from the medial occipital surface over the first 1 or 2 cm of the posterior surface of the occipital lobe. The main blood supply of the V1 area derives from the posterior cerebral artery. A parieto-occipital branch supplies the superior bank of the calcarine fissure, and a posterior temporal branch supplies its inferior bank. A calcarine branch also supplies the central region. The occipital pole is a watershed zone between the posterior and middle cerebral arteries, with significant individual variation in the supply of the foveal representation within V1.

Fig. 2. Human visual cortex. Brodmann (1909) divided the human cortex into several different cytoarchitectonic areas. He designated striate cortex as area 17 and showed it as symmetrically surrounding the calcarine fissure on the mesial surface of the hemispheres. Simian area V1 is located more laterally (see Fig. 1). The foveal representation is located toward the occipital pole, while the peripheral visual field representations are located anteriorly, in the depths of the calcarine fissure. The parieto-occipital fissure is a fairly reliable anterior boundary for dorsal area 17. Extrastriate areas 18 and 19 concentrically surround area 17 and should correspond to the monkey's areas V2 and V3. Top. Ventral 18 and 19 (including the fusiform and lingual gyri) and adjacent temporal lobe areas contribute to a “what” pathway, which should contain regions corresponding to simian areas V4 (dark oval area) and IT. Bottom. Dorsal 18 and 19 extend onto the lateral surface of the hemisphere and contribute to a parietal or “where” pathway, which may contain a human homologue of the monkey's area MT (dark oval area) in portions of 19, 37, and 39.

Inouye17 and Holmes and Lister18 found that in area 17 the foveal representation of the retina is located near the occipital pole, and the peripheral visual field is represented anteriorly. These insights came from studying the pattern of visual field defects in soldiers with penetrating wounds of the occiput. Further study has revealed a well-defined and orderly retinotopic arrangement of the visual field in area V1.19 The superior bank of the calcarine fissure contains cortex corresponding to the superior retina (inferior visual field), while the inferior bank contains the representation of the superior visual field. Most anterior is the representation of the monocular temporal crescent, the region of temporal field in the contralateral eye that lies beyond the limits of the nasal field (60°) of the ipsilateral eye. As is true of most of the visual system, the peripheral field is less represented than central vision. In V1, more than one half of cortex is devoted to the central 10° of vision (“cortical magnification”). Occipital cortex contains a mixture of monocular and binocular cells arranged in ocular dominance columns; however, there is no gross separation of ocular inputs.

VISUAL FIELD DEFECTS FROM STRIATE LESIONS

Focal destruction of the visual cortex produces a scotoma, defined as a region of the visual field within which the patient cannot reliably report the presence of targets of specific size and luminance, tested with either kinetic targets (e.g., Goldmann perimetry20) or static targets (e.g., threshold automated perimetry [Allergan, 1987]). These defects are located in the visual field contralateral to an area V1 lesion and are homonymous, being present in similar regions of the visual fields of both eyes. Their homonymous nature reflects the decussation of the nasal retinal fibers at the optic chiasm. As opposed to visual defects caused by lesions of the optic radiations and especially by lesions of the optic tracts, the hemianopic defects from V1 lesions are highly congruent, meaning that the scotoma or defect in one eye is virtually identical to that in the other eye (Figs. 3 to 7). This reflects the anatomic convergence of the inputs of one eye with that of the other in area V1.

Fig. 3. Macular-sparing homonymous hemianopia. Visual field by Goldmann perimetry (isopter to V4e target) in a 49-year-old woman with a right medial occipital infarct sparing the occipital pole.

Fig. 4. Right inferior quadrantanopia sparing the macula but not respecting the horizontal meridian. Visual field by Goldmann perimetry (isopter to V4e target) in a 50-year-old woman with a striate infarct involving mainly the superior bank of the calcarine fissure.

Fig. 5. Homonymous right inferior central scotomata. Visual field by Goldmann perimetry (isopter to V4e target) in a 38-year-old man with sudden complaints of difficulty reading. MRI showed an infarct of the left occipital pole.

Fig. 6. Homonymous right inferior paracentral scotomata. Visual field by Goldmann perimetry (isopter to V4e target) in a 14-year-old girl with perinatal occipital infarction. MRI showed a lesion of the midportion of the left superior calcarine bank, sparing both the occipital pole and the anterior striate cortex.

Fig. 7. Macular-splitting hemianopia with sparing of the monocular temporal crescent. Visual field by Goldmann perimetry (isopter to V4e target) in a 66-year-old man with a right medial occipital lobe infarct.

With a complete V1 lesion, the entire contralateral visual hemifield is blind, including one half of foveal vision. This is referred to as a macula-splitting homonymous hemianopia. When vascular in origin, this often represents a posterior cerebral artery infarct in a patient whose entire calcarine cortex was supplied by that artery. Macula-splitting hemianopias, however, also occur with complete lesions of the optic tract or optic radiations and therefore lack localizing value. In contrast, a patient whose macular pole is either supplied by or has abundant collateral circulation from the middle cerebral artery may have a macula-sparing hemianopia (Fig. 3).21 Previously it had been argued that macula-sparing results from bilateral representation of a small region surrounding the vertical meridian, which expands to as much as 3° at the fovea22; however, a recent study of the retinotopy of V1 in the monkey found that the hemi-maculae are not represented bilaterally.23 In contrast to macula-splitting hemianopias, macula-sparing hemianopias are rarely due to lesions of the optic radiation or optic tract.

Partial lesions of area V1 are not uncommon. Involvement of either the upper or lower bank alone usually results from ischemia, since the banks are supplied by separate branches. Upper bank infarcts cause homonymous contralateral inferior quadrantanopia (Fig. 4), whereas lower bank infarcts cause superior quadrantanopia. Such defects often do not respect the horizontal meridian (see Fig. 4), since the representation of the upper and lower fields is continuous across the meridian in the depths of the calcarine fissure. Similar quadrantanopias can be found with parietal or temporal lobe lesions involving the optic radiations. Partial involvement can also occur along the anteroposterior extent of striate cortex. A lesion of the occipital pole alone can cause a hemianopic central scotoma (Fig. 5),24 whereas a lesion slightly more anterior can cause a hemianopic paracentral scotoma (Fig. 6). These defects are distinguished from ocular causes of central visual loss by their highly congruent and homonymous nature and their respect of the vertical meridian. These central hemianopias sometimes result from watershed infarcts during systemic hypoperfusion. A near-complete lesion that spares only the most anterior portion of V1 may cause a hemianopia that initially appears incongruous (Fig. 7). The hemianopia involves the whole nasal hemifield of the ipsilateral eye, but the temporal hemianopia of the contralateral eye is remarkable for a preserved crescent-shaped island of remnant vision.25 This is the monocular temporal crescent, the sole region of the visual field that is represented only in one eye. This apparent incongruity may be attributed mistakenly to an optic tract lesion; however, the absence of both optic atrophy and relative afferent pupillary defect (RAPD), along with the location of the crescent outside 60° and the high congruity of the homonymous defect inside 60°, indicates a striate lesion. In fact, hemianopia with sparing of the temporal crescent is pathognomonic of a V1 lesion.

Bilateral lesions of striate cortex are not infrequent. Because both the right and left striate cortex face each other on the medial occipital surface, localized pathologic processes such as tumors or traumatic injury may affect both sides concurrently. Also, because the right and left posterior cerebral arteries share a common origin from the basilar artery, vascular disease also can affect both visual areas either simultaneously or sequentially. Distinction from bilateral optic nerve or ocular pathology relies on the high congruity of the visual fields and the demonstration of step defects along the vertical meridian that betray the bilateral homonymous nature of the field loss. With bilateral hemiscotomata from occipital pole lesions, however, such steps may be difficult to demonstrate and require skillful perimetry26 to avoid confusion with macular disease or optic neuropathy.

Cortical Blindness and Anton's Syndrome

Complete bilateral striate lesions lead to cortical blindness. Cortical blindness is easily distinguished from ocular disease by the normal pupillary responses to light and normal funduscopic examination. Some patients with cortical blindness appear unaware of their deficit and deny that they cannot see (Anton's syndrome). The cause of Anton's syndrome remains obscure. Some have suggested that it has a common origin with other anosognosic syndromes,8 in which patients deny the presence of hemiplegia or other neurologic problems, and which are thought to arise from right hemispheric dysfunction or disconnections between the thalamus and the right parietal lobe.27 Denial of blindness is not pathognomonic of cortical blindness. It can also occur in subjects with ocular or optic nerve disease; however, in these patients lack of awareness seems to be related to concurrent dementia or confusional states.28 In contrast, denial of blindness with lesions of the visual cortex is not necessarily accompanied by delirium or dementia. Thus it may be better to define Anton's syndrome more specifically as denial of blindness in the absence of dementia or delirium. Even this more restricted definition, however, is not exclusive to lesions of visual cortex, since Anton's syndrome occasionally is mimicked by a combination of bilateral optic neuropathy and bilateral frontal lobe disease.29

Inverse Anton's syndrome is a more unusual condition in which patients with incomplete visual loss deny any ability to see. In contrast to patients with “blindsight,” these patients have small islands of preserved vision that can be demonstrated, and there has been at least one case in which neuroimaging demonstrated residual striate cortex corresponding to the remnant visual field. Hartmann and associates30 suggested that this “meaning stripped of its percept” (contrasting with Teuber's31 famous definition of agnosia as a percept stripped of its meaning) resulted from disconnection of visual perception in striate cortex from attentional mechanisms in the parietal lobes. Inverse Anton's syndrome remains a controversial entity, however, and further work is required to verify its existence and elucidate its pathophysiology.

BLINDSIGHT AND RESIDUAL VISION

One of the most interesting questions regarding the visual field defects caused by striate lesions is whether any residual vision exists within the homonymous scotoma. Although there are older investigations of remnant visual function within cortical visual field defects, the modern quantitative study of this phenomenon begins with Pöppel and colleagues,32 who reported some rudimentary localization of targets within hemianopic regions by saccadic eye movements. After this report, Weiskrantz33,34 performed a series of studies on patient D.B., who had a surgical resection of an occipital lobe.35 D.B. always denied seeing visual stimuli in the hemianopic field, but when forced to guess, he was able to make a variety of visual discriminations at a level better than chance. In other words, D.B. seemed to show visual perception in the absence of acknowledged awareness, a phenomenon that Weiskrantz labeled “blindsight.” Blindsight should be distinguished from “residual vision,” another phenomenon that is sometimes discussed together with blindsight. Patients with residual vision retain some awareness of the presence or absence of visual stimuli within a perimetrically defined visual field defect.36–38 Thus, their hemianopia is relative even if severe, and the finding of some residual discriminative abilities is less surprising in these patients. Whether blindsight and residual vision share the same mechanisms is unclear.

Measuring Remnant Abilities in Blindsight

Blindsight is not a commonly reported phenomenon, despite heightened awareness of the condition in recent years. It is clear that not all subjects with cortical field defects exhibit blindsight. Weiskrantz39 suggested that this resulted from variable inclusion of extrastriate regions within human lesions. Another possibly important factor is age of onset. Children or infants may be more likely to develop blindsight, since they may be at a stage of greater neural plasticity.36,40 Demonstrating blindsight also requires special assessment strategies beyond those in common use in the eye clinic.41,42 These include (1) measurement of eye movements or manual pointing as an index of the spatial localization of visual targets; and (2) forced-choice methods that circumvent the need for subjects to assert, “Yes, I saw it,” the response required in typical perimetric tests. The question raised by these special techniques, however, is whether the improvement observed with forced-choice strategies indicates subconscious vision or merely a relaxation of the criteria used by a subject to respond.43 Indeed, normal subjects shown very brief stimuli exhibit similar patterns of response to those of blindsight patients.44 Recent studies have addressed this problem by using signal detection analysis, which calculates a measure of true sensitivity (d') independent of a response criterion.41,45

Questions have often been raised as to whether blindsight results are due to artifact, such as inadequate fixation, light scatter, nonvisual cues, and nonrandom presentation of targets.43 Inadequate fixation is controlled best by monitoring eye position, although the use of randomly located, very brief stimulus presentations may also minimize this concern. Even so, eye recordings may not detect sustained strategies of eccentric fixation in patients, unless rigorous methods of head stabilization are used.46 Light scatter is particularly troublesome, since Campion and co-workers43 showed that blindsight-like performances could be mimicked by light scatter. Some investigators have measured scatter physically37,47; however, Campion and associates43 argued that psychophysical rather than physical measures are required. Some studies have used anatomic controls for scatter, such as showing that similar stimuli at the physiologic blind spot are not perceived34; other studies have used control patients with pregeniculate lesions, the expectation being that such lesions eliminate the pathways for blindsight.32,48 All of these issues point to the fact that the demonstration of blindsight requires extreme care in guarding against artifactual results.

The range of visual ability demonstrated in the literature is impressive, including perception of spatial location, form, orientation, color, and motion (Table 1). No clear pattern of preserved versus eradicated visual perception has emerged. In part, this may reflect the idiosyncratic selection of tasks by investigators and the impossibility of testing all aspects of perception with the time-consuming methodologies required in cases of blindsight.

 

TABLE ONE. Residual Visual Abilities Shown in Various Blindsight Studies


  Remnant Visual Ability Demonstrated
 No. of PatientsSpatial LocalizationContrast Sensitivity
  SaccadesManualMotionFormColor 
Patients with Focal Cerebral Lesions       
ter Braak et al, 19711  (OKN)   
Pöppel et al, 19734+     
Sanders et al, 19741 (DB)+     
Weiskrantz et al, 19741 (DB)++ +(-) 
Perenin and Jeannerod, 19756++ (-)  
Torjussen, 19783   +  
Perenin et al, 19801 ++   
Barbur et al, 19801 (GY)  +(-) +
Meienberg et al, 19813(-)     
Bridgeman and Staggs, 19821 +    
Zihl, 1980; Zihl and Werth, 19846+     
Pizzamiglio et al, 19841  +   
Blythe et al, 19862 (GY)+     
Blythe et al, 19872 (GY)+++(-)+ 
Stoerig, 19876    + 
Hess and Pointer, 19893 (GY)     (-)
Corbetta et al, 19901 +    
Heide et al, 19902  +   
Stoerig and Cowey, 19913    + 
Perenin, 19915  (-OKN)   
Mestre et al, 19921  +(-)  
Patients with Hemidecortication       
Perenin and Jeannerod, 19786 +    
Perenin, 19786   +  
Ptito et al, 19874   +  
Ptito et al, 19914 ++   
Braddick et al, 19922+     

DB = patient DB (subjects in study); GY = patient GY; (OKN) optokinetic nystagmus; + = positive finding; - = negative finding.

 

Saccadic Localization

Spatial localization has been one of the most extensively studied of blindsight abilities, reflecting expectations that functions of the superior colliculus, such as localization, would be the most likely to remain after loss of striate cortex.49 Localization has been studied in both saccadic and manual pointing responses. Pöppel and associates32 reported a weak correlation of saccadic size and target position in four patients with incomplete hemianopias, mainly for targets up to 30° in eccentricity. Similarly, patient D.B.33,35 had only a weak correlation of eye responses between 5° and 25°; for the entire range, the correlation was not significant. Most of the correlation derived from reduced saccadic size for the nearest target at 5°, in a portion of the visual field that later recovered on perimetry.34 Perenin and Jeannerod48 found some saccadic localization in two patients with cortical hemianopia, again mainly with targets less than 30°. Lack of correlation at greater eccentricities in these studies perhaps reflects the fact that centrifugal head-fixed saccades of greater than 30° are rare under natural circumstances. In contrast to these reports, Meienberg and colleagues50 did not find any localization ability in three subjects with cerebral hemianopia.

Manual Localization

Studies of localization by reaching and pointing allow a greater range of eccentricities to be studied than do studies of head-fixed saccades. Despite the poor saccadic localization of patient D.B., Weiskrantz and associates33 found near-normal manual localization with large targets beyond 30°, again a region that showed recovery in later perimetry.34 Perenin and Jeannerod48 reported some localization ability in six patients with postgeniculate lesions; however, they did not quantify data, and their findings were complicated by the fact that subjects reported seeing a flash, implying either light scatter or conventional vision. Scatter may be particularly important, given that the localization occurred mainly for points of less than 20° eccentricity. In a later study of a cortically blind patient, manual localization was weak and variable.47 Corbetta and co-workers51 found a similar weak correlation of pointing with target location in only one of four patients. Blythe and associates36 reported some manual localization, but only in patients who were aware of the target (i.e., residual vision), which excludes the presence of blindsight by definition.

Manual localization in hemidecorticate patients also has been studied.49,52 Again, the correlation between pointing and target position in six patients of Perenin and Jeannerod49 was variable and appeared to derive mainly from the nearer targets. Ptito and colleagues52 found that localization ability was spread over more of the field, although it remained more variable and less accurate than in the normal hemifield. These subjects were aware of targets, suggesting the presence of residual vision, rather than blindsight.

Motion

Some studies of blindsight have tested spatial localization with moving or oscillating targets.32,33,37,47,49,52,53 Several of these studies have suggested an advantage of localization for moving over stationary targets.49,53 Better performance with moving targets is reminiscent of Riddoch's54 phenomenon, in which appreciation of movement precedes the appreciation of static targets in recovering hemianopic field defects.

Discriminations of motion speed or direction have also been reported in cases of blindsight. Perenin55 found residual direction discrimination in five patients with hemianopia but sparing of the lateral occipitotemporal cortex. Heide and co-workers56 found that 2 of 10 subjects discriminated direction and experienced some self-motion from gratings in the blind field. Patient G.Y. (who had residual vision, rather than blindsight) had poor direction discrimination but could detect bright moving targets and discriminate speed differences.36,37 Ptito and associates52 reported that two of three hemidecorticate patients were able to detect motion and had some speed discrimination, but not direction discrimination; however, their results might be explained by response biases of the subjects, rather than blindsight.

Other motion-related abilities have been described. Ter Braak and colleagues57 reported on a man who had recovery of optokinetic nystagmus 5 months after a stroke that caused permanent cortical blindness. Pizzamiglio and co-workers58 found that rotating stimuli in the blind hemifield exerted an effect on the subject's judgment of the true vertical; however, their stimuli may have inadvertently provided clues that influenced responses. Mestre and associates59 studied perception of optic flow in a man with bilateral occipital infarcts. In his completely blind right hemifield, the subject could detect optic flow, discriminate speed differences, and tell the difference between forward and backward flow. However, their display may have abutted an island of preserved vision in the left hemifield, suggesting that scatter or inadvertent stimulation within this island was responsible for his good performance.

Form and Color

Weiskrantz and colleagues33 first reported that patient D.B. had normal discrimination of the form (X vs O) of large stimuli and later concluded that this was explained by orientation discrimination.34 In contrast, patient G.Y. had poor orientation and pattern discrimination.37 Perenin and Jeannerod's study48 of six patients and Blythe and co-workers' study36 of five patients (including G.Y.) with residual vision also found no pattern discrimination with postgeniculate lesions. The patient of Mestre and associates59 also could not distinguish X from O. Torjussen60 reported that three patients with residual vision could distinguish semicircles from complete circles, despite the fact that the completing half of the circle lay in the blind hemifield. However, all three had hemianopias that spared the macula, suggesting that sufficient clues may have been present in seeing regions.43

Form perception has also been studied in hemidecorticate patients.40,61 Perenin40 suggested some discrimination between triangles and disks in two of six patients, though only one showed consistent results across trials. Performance was better near the vertical meridian, raising the specter of light scatter. Ptito and colleagues61 found some form discrimination in one of four patients, with some curious features: Discrimination was possible (1) with three-dimensional objects, but not with two-dimensional objects; and (2) only when one stimulus was in a blind hemifield and the other in a seeing hemifield, but not when both stimuli were in the blind hemifield.

The range of residual functions in blindsight now also includes color. Blythe and co-workers36 reported that one of five patients with residual vision after occipital lesions could detect and identify red and green. Stoerig45 found that 6 of 10 patients detected red and green targets but not targets of similar wavelength composition to the background (i.e., achromatic). A more recent study of spectral sensitivity in three patients found evidence for color-opponent processes in the blind hemifield.42 This contrasts with G.Y.'s spectral sensitivity curves, which were scotopic in shape,37 and the fact that D.B.'s ability to distinguish red from green fell to chance when brightness was varied.33

Hess and Pointer62 tested spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity with forced-choice methods in three hemianopic patients, including G.Y. The rate of correct answers was no better than chance. This contrasts with previous claims of residual vision in G.Y.37 and raises comparisons with the method-of-adjustment techniques used in the prior study.

Interactions between blind and normal hemifields have been suggested. In three hemianopic patients, Rafal and associates63 found that distractor targets in the blind hemifield prolonged the latency of saccades, but not manual reactions to targets in the intact field. Marzi and colleagues64 and Corbetta and co-workers51 studied spatial and temporal summation effects across the vertical meridian. Corbetta and associates51 found one subject who had evidence of blindsight in all tasks; however, in other subjects in these two studies, the results were inconsistent.

Effects of Training

In six subjects, Zihl and Werth65–67 found that a visual localization training program improved the accuracy of a series of saccades in a visual search task. It even has been claimed that training actually leads to an expansion of visual fields.68,69 Improvement in manual localization with training also was found in one case of bilateral blindness.53 Blythe and colleagues, however, did not find a training effect for the accuracy of single saccades.36 Balliet and co-workers46 also found no such improvement in 12 patients, and they suggested that positive effects may reflect learned strategies of eccentric fixation.

Anatomy of Blindsight

Initially the favored hypothesis was that blindsight was a “second visual system” involving the superior colliculus. Indeed, in cases of hemidecortication, this must remain the primary hypothesis. The retinotopic arrangement of collicular receptive fields and the discharge of collicular cells with saccades guided early studies of saccadic localization in hemianopic patients.32 The discovery of other remnant visual abilities such as pattern and motion perception, however, are not easily explained by collicular activity alone. These led to suggestions that collicular projections to the pulvinar may provide an indirect input to extrastriate cortex.33 This would require some adaptation or “plasticity,” since the visual responses in the pulvinar normally originate from the visual cortex and not from the superior colliculus.70,71 Weiskrantz34,39 attributed variations in the degree and type of blindsight possessed by subjects to the inevitable variability in naturally occurring human lesions. Most lesions of striate cortex involve some extrastriate regions as well, but the areas affected and the extent of damage are different in each patient. Weiskrantz proposed that the type of blindsight reflects the pattern of extrastriate sparing; however, some studies have not found a correlation between blindsight and lesion extent as depicted on a scan (e.g., MRI).64,72 More recently the colliculopulvinocortical relay has been further challenged by demonstrations of color perception in blindsight.42,45 Since collicular neurons lack color opponency, Cowey and Stoerig73 proposed that there is a projection to extrastriate cortex from lateral geniculate neurons that survives the retrograde degeneration occurring after a striate lesion.

The early impetus for human studies on blindsight followed demonstrations of residual vision in primates after striate resection.74 These animals retain or relearn the ability to localize targets with saccades.75–77 They can also detect and discriminate the speed of moving targets,76 but there is disagreement over whether they can use motion to guide smooth pursuit and saccades.77,78 Saccadic localization in destriated monkeys is lost when the striate lesion is combined with a collicular one.75 In contrast, little effect is seen when striate lesions are enlarged to include extrastriate areas,76 supporting the hypothesis that remnant vision depends on the superior colliculus alone. Single-cell studies, however, do show that some activity remains in extrastriate areas after the occurrence of striate lesions. In particular, responses remain in areas MT and V3A ,79–82 components of the dorsal pathway of visual processing. This activity is lost after further ablation of the superior colliculus,83 supporting the collicular-relay hypothesis advocated by Weiskrantz.34,39 However, the activity in components of the ventral pathway, such as V381 and inferior temporal cortex,79 is abolished by striate lesions. Since perception of color and form are commonly attributed to this ventral stream, these findings appear to conflict with demonstrations of form and color discrimination in human blindsight.

Not all investigators believe, however, that blindsight results from collicular or extrastriate function. The possibility that blindsight depends on remnant striate cortex32,43 is still viable, given the difficulty of proving complete destruction without performing an autopsy. Fendrich and associates84 used highly detailed perimetry in one “blindsight” case to find a small island of preserved vision, which corresponded to spared striate cortex visualized with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). It is also not clear whether “residual vision” is due to nonstriate function: Celesia and colleagues85 found that conscious perception of stimuli requires residual striate cortex, although Ptito and co-workers52 claimed that their patients with hemispherectomies retained some awareness of visual targets. Some monkey data also suggest that remnant vision requires intact striate cortex: Merigan and associates85a found no remnant vision within a scotoma after a V1 lesion, but they did detect some in a V2 scotoma. Thus, at present, the anatomy and physiology of blindsight remain uncertain.73,86

Back to Top
LESIONS OF EXTRASTRIATE CORTEX
Area 17 or V1 is concentrically surrounded by extrastriate (prestriate) areas 18 and 19. These areas likely correspond to the monkey's area V2 and V3. Areas 18 and 19 are mostly medial: their ventromedial aspects include portions of the fusiform and lingual gyri and may correspond to another visual area of the monkey, area V4. Like V1, the ventromedial parts of areas 18 and 19 are supplied by branches of the posterior cerebral artery; however, areas 18 and 19 also extend onto the lateral surface of the occipital lobe. Several studies suggest that a human homologue of the monkey's area MT (V5) complex is present on the lateral surface of the brain, in a region where the occipital, temporal, and parietal lobes converge. This would include portions of Brodmann's areas 19, 37, and 39. Like the occipital pole, the blood supply of these lateral regions falls in the watershed zone between the middle and posterior cerebral arteries.

Given our knowledge of parallel visual processing in the extrastriate cortex of monkeys, an attempt at broadly categorizing the visual dysfunction from human lesions can be made. Lesions of extrastriate cortex fall into two categories: (1) those affecting components of the ventral pathway, which disrupt form and color perception; and (2) those affecting components of the dorsal pathway, which disrupt motion and visuospatial perception.

VENTRAL PATHWAY

Visual Agnosia and Prosopagnosia

The word agnosia, derived from the Greek ( = perception), means “not knowing.” Associative agnosia is an impairment of recognition in which “normal percepts are stripped of their meanings.31 Apperceptive agnosia is a failure of recognition due to impaired perceptual abilities.87

One of the most striking agnosic disturbances is prosopagnosia (Gr. pros<ovrbaro>pon = face), the inability to recognize familiar faces despite adequate visual abilities. The term was coined by Bodamer87a in 1947, although the deficit was described much earlier by Quaglino and Borelli in88 The difficulty involves recognition of faces familiar before the start of the illness (retrograde prosopagnosia) as well as the ability to learn new faces (anterograde prosopagnosia). Because abnormal facial recognition may be part of more generalized perceptual, cognitive, or memory problems,89 the diagnosis of prosopagnosia is best reserved for cases in which the recognition defect appears disproportionately severe.

Prosopagnosic patients usually complain of the embarrassing social consequences of their problem, although those with childhood onset90,91 and the odd adult case92 may be unaware of their defect. To recognize others, they rely on nonvisual cues, such as the context of an encounter or the sound of a voice; or visual cues, such as gait, clothing, or even local facial features, such as hair length, glasses, or facial hair.90 Use of these visual cues, however, does not constitute true facial recognition.

Despite their problems with facial identity, many patients can determine age, gender, and emotional expression from faces93–96 or can even lip read.97 The process by which they do so, however, may differ from normal. For example, one patient could no longer tell age when wrinkles were removed from pictures of faces, whereas a normal subject was still able to judge age accurately.90 In some cases involving more pervasive perceptual abnormalities, judgments about age, sex, and expression are also affected.90,91

An important issue is whether the defect underlying prosopagnosia is specific for faces or extends to other objects. Some patients have difficulty distinguishing among types of an object, such as types of cars, flowers, food, or coins90,91,93–95,98–100; or among unique items, such as buildings, handwriting, personal belongings, and clothing.90,96,98,100 At a broader level, though, prosopagnosics have no difficulty recognizing a face as a face, a cow as a cow, or a car as a car. Some authors have suggested that this implies difficulty making distinctions within a category of objects, of which faces are merely the most dramatic example, whereas distinctions between object categories are preserved.100 Others have pointed out, however, that determining what constitutes a category is an arbitrary, somewhat intuitive decision on the part of investigator.101 In any case, there is clearly variability in the degree to which prosopagnosia affects the recognition of other objects. De Renzi's patient102 recognized his own razor, wallet, glasses, handwriting, and car. The patient of Bruyer and associates93 recognized individual cows, dogs, houses, streets, and cars, and his only difficulty with playing cards involved face cards. McNeil and Warrington103 describe a man who learned to distinguish among individual sheep after the onset of his prosopagnosia, a more daunting task than recognizing human faces for normal subjects.

Almost all prosopagnosic patients have other visual problems (Table 2). Visual field defects are frequent, the most common being left homonymous hemianopia, or upper quadrantanopia in the left or both hemifields.92,95,104–108 Visual acuity is only sometimes reduced,90,91,104 and spatial contrast sensitivity may be impaired or normal.90,91,109 Achromatopsia or hemiachromatopsia90,92,93,104–107,110 and topographagnosia90–92,95,104,110 form a common triad with prosopagnosia, but there are cases of patients with normal color vision.92,94,95,106,108 Visual object agnosia is said to be absent in some,95,104,106,108 but present though proportionately milder in others.90,91,94 More severe generalized apperceptive visual agnosia would preclude a specific diagnosis of prosopagnosia. Another frequent finding is impaired visual memory93,94,104,106,107,110; some subjects also have impaired verbal memory.92–94,106 Other occasionally associated defects include simultanagnosia,93,94 palinopsia,92 visual hallucinations,92 and left-neglect, hemisensory defects or hemiparesis in those with right unilateral lesions.92 The determining factor for these last associated defects is likely the extent of the causative brain lesion.

 

TABLE TWO. Associated Signs in Prosopagnosia

  Major Signs
  Visual field defect

  Left homonymous hemianopia
  Left superior quadrantanopia
  Bilateral superior quadrantanopia


  Achromatopsia, hemiachromatopsia
  Topographagnosia
  Minor Signs
  Visual object agnosia
  Visual amnesia
  Occasional Signs
  Simultanagnosia
  Palinopsia
  Visual hallucinations
  Left hemineglect
  Left hemisensory loss
  Left hemiparesis

 

ANATOMY AND PATHOLOGY. The typical lesion in prosopagnosic cases lies in the inferior occipitotemporal cortex, in the fusiform and lingual gyri (Fig. 8).100,111,112 Opinion has varied as to whether prosopagnosia requires bilateral lesions. The early cases of Wilbrand (1892) and Heidenhain (1927) had bilateral damage.89 Reviews by Meadows112 and Damasio and colleagues100 stressed that all autopsies of prosopagnosic patients revealed bilateral lesions of the inferior occipitotemporal cortex; in some cases, the left-sided lesion was not suspected before the patient died.98,113 Other evidence for the requirement of bilateral lesions include studies of split-brain subjects showing mechanisms for facial recognition in both hemispheres and the lack of prosopagnosia in patients with right hemispherectomy89 (see Sergent and Villemure95 for an exception). Modern neuroimaging has also confirmed bilateral lesions in many cases with computerized tomography (CT),93,96,104,106,107 MRI,89,106 and positron-emission tomography (PET).114

Fig. 8. The brain of a 72-year-old woman with prosopagnosia, achromatopsia, alexia, and left homonymous hemianopia. The mesial surfaces of the right and left hemispheres, respectively, are reconstructed in the upper right and upper left panels from raw MRI data (Brainvox technique102). The ventral surface is in the lower left panel. The white lines through the reconstructed brain show the relative position of the four coronal slices shown in the lower right panel. The calcarine fissure and parieto-occipital fissure are traced in red and yellow, respectively, which automatically transfers to the coronal MRI slices. The patient had bilateral lesions that affected the fusiform gyrus and undercut the most posterior segment of the lingual gyrus. The larger lesion in the right hemisphere affected the optic radiations, causing the equivalent of a V1 scotoma in the left visual hemifield. The lesion of the left hemisphere does not reach the surface of the brain. It lies beneath the calcarine fissure and can be only in the coronal sections. Such a lesion would cause damage in a possible human homologue of the monkey's area V4 complex or disrupt connections to and from such an area. (Rizzo M, Smith V, Pokorny J, Damasio AR: Color perception profiles in central achromatopsia. Neurology 43:995, 1993)

Damasio and co-workers100 stated that the bilateral lesions in prosopagnosia were often symmetric, presumably affecting homologous regions of both hemispheres, whereas Meadows112 believed the position of the left lesion was more variable. Others have argued from this variability that the bilateral nature of the damage is compatible with right-sided lateralization of facial recognition.102,115 Left-sided lesions may disconnect a right hemispheric locus for facial recognition from visual input of the right hemifield, which would be especially critical in cases with left hemianopia.99,116,117 More recently, reports of CT scans showing apparently unilateral right occipitotemporal lesions have accumulated.92,95,118,119 Although Damasio89 suggested that CT scans may miss small lesions in the left hemisphere, there has been at least one case in which a unilateral lesion was documented with an MRI scan.120 Moreover, in one autopsy case, Landis and colleagues121 found only a trivial and probably unimportant lesion on the left. Thus there is mounting evidence that prosopagnosia may develop after the occurrence of right-sided lesions of the fusiform gyrus alone.88 Whether the mechanism, severity, or type of prosopagnosia differs between patients with unilateral and bilateral lesions remains to be determined.

The most common lesions causing prosopagnosia arise from head trauma,92,94,104,105,110,122 posterior cerebral artery infarctions,92,93,100,103,107,118,123,124 and to a lesser extent viral encephalitis.96,122,123 These conditions all have the potential to cause bilateral damage. Other unilateral lesions, such as primary tumors,92 hematomas,92,108 abscesses,122 and surgical resections,95,104 are reported less frequently. Prosopagnosia may rarely occur with focal bilateral degeneration in the elderly114 and as a developmental disorder in the young,91 analogous to the reading disorder known as developmental dyslexia.

COVERT VERSUS OVERT RECOGNITION. Although prosopagnosic patients deny being familiar with faces and cannot identify them, it appears that some “knowledge” of faces is retained in some patients. A range of physiologic and behavioral techniques have been used to uncover this “covert recognition.”125 The type of retained knowledge shown falls into two categories: (1) distinguishing familiar from unfamiliar faces; and (2) demonstrating knowledge pertaining to an individual face, such as name and occupation.

Covert familiarity was demonstrated with electrodermal skin conductance measurements by Tranel and Damasio.123 They showed two patients a series of familiar and unfamiliar faces and found larger amplitude responses to the familiar faces. Renault and associates108 used the amplitude of the P300 component of visual evoked potentials to show covert familiarity in one case. Behavioral methods also demonstrate covert familiarity. Rizzo and colleagues106 studied the eye movements of the two patients reported by Tranel and Damasio.123 When normal subjects scan familiar faces they concentrate on internal facial features (i.e., eyes, nose, mouth) rather than external ones, whereas both external and internal features of unknown faces are examined. This pattern was also shown by the prosopagnosic patients, despite their inability to state which faces were familiar. Normal subjects are quicker at judging whether two faces are from the same person or not when the faces are familiar, and this difference depends on internal facial features. A similar difference between familiar and unfamiliar faces in reaction time and reliance on inner features was shown in one prosopagnosic patient.94 Likewise, for both normal subjects and one prosopagnosic subject, Sergent and Poncet96 demonstrated that matching the old and young faces of persons across a 30-year gap was also easier with famous faces than with unknown ones.

Both physiologic and behavioral methods have also shown covert knowledge of information about faces. When prosopagnosic patients saw famous faces and were read a list of names, there were more electrodermal skin responses when correct names were heard.107,110 When forced to choose between a correct and incorrect name for a face, one patient guessed better than chance,96 though another did not when the choice concerned occupation.94 Several prosopagnosic patients were better at learning to associate a familiar face with a correct name than with an incorrect name.93,94,96,126

MECHANISMS. In the past, prosopagnosia was attributed to a combination of generalized mental impairment and disturbances of perception and memory.127,128 It is clear now that it is a specific functional disorder with a specific neuroanatomic basis, as proposed by Hoff and Potzl129 and Bodamer.87a Much of the early debate about its specific nature centered on whether it was a failure of perception or of memory, a dichotomy resembling the apperceptive versus associative distinction for agnosia described by Lissauer.87 The fact that many prosopagnosic subjects can make “same or different” judgments about faces, even with different lighting conditions or views (i.e., Benton Facial Recognition Test88), led to conclusions that perception was adequate and therefore the defect was one of memory. It is becoming more apparent, however, that prosopagnosia may not be just one disorder but a group of disorders, with varying degrees of perceptual and memory dysfunction.122

The defect (or defects) underlying prosopagnosia have been conceptualized in neuropsychologic terms. Both Damasio89 and Bruce and Young130 have proposed a staged process of facial recognition:

  Stage 1: Perception or structural encoding of a facial percept. This provides data not only for facial recognition, but also for analysis of facial expression, speech, age, and sex.
  Stage 2: Activation of facial recognition units or templates by sufficiently similar facial percepts from stage 1.
  Stage 3: Activated facial recognition units that access personal identity nodes containing biographic information and multimodal memories about the owner of the face. These memories can also be accessed through other routes besides facial recognition, such as recognition of voice or gait, which are often preserved in prosopagnosic patients.

Tranel and Damasio123 originally suggested that the prosopagnosic defect was faulty activation of memories (i.e., personal identity nodes) by templates (i.e., facial recognition units). This “associative” prosopagnosia would correspond with the memory type of prosopagnosia proposed by de Renzi and co-workers.122 Damasio89 subsequentlysuggested that prosopagnosia might arise from defects at any level in the process. Thus there may be defects in complex higher order visual analyses of facial features, percept-matching to templates, or template activation of memories. Defects of visual analysis, or structural encoding of faces, would correspond to the “apperceptive” type of prosopagnosia of de Renzi and associates.122

The nature of the defect in apperceptive prosopagnosia has been studied by Sergent and Villemure95 and Levine and Calvanio.131 They have suggested that apperceptive prosopagnosia is caused by abnormal configural processing, meaning an inability to perceive the whole from the component features. Levine and Calvanio's131 patient performed poorly on “Gestalt completion” tests of visual closure; Sergent and Villemure's95 patient with apperceptive prosopagnosia used a feature-by-feature method rather than the whole-form strategy used by both normal subjects and a second patient with associative prosopagnosia in a later study.96 In a similar vein, Rentschler and colleagues'132 prosopagnosic patient could not perceive texture or Moiré patterns despite having intact perception of textural elements, suggesting a disturbance of “global” visual integration.

Disturbances of Color Processing

CEREBRAL ACHROMATOPSIA. Cerebral achromatopsia is an acquired defect of color perception caused by bilateral lesions of visual cortex or its connections. Patients complain that the world appears colorless, in shades of gray,133–137 or less bright.137,138 Daily activities that rely on color discrimination are affected, such as distinguishing coins, stamps, or traffic lights (the experience of an achromatopsic artist has been described by Sacks139). A few patients will note other color phenomena, such as abnormal coloration of the world, as if seeing through a colored filter,140 and illusory spread of residual colors beyond object boundaries.135 These latter ancillary phenomena may occur transiently as the achromatopsic syndrome evolves after an acute event.

Achromatopsia is frequently accompanied by other signs (Table 3). Superior quadrantanopia is almost always present, either in both hemifields or in one hemifield with complete hemianopia in the other (for an exception, see Victor and associates141). Prosopagnosia is also almost always present, and topographagnosia is frequent.135,141,142 Other types of object agnosia also occur.142,143 Occasionally patients have some memory loss due to extension of lesions into the anterior temporal lobes.135,143 Alexia without agraphia may be present in achromatopsic patients with right homonymous hemianopias.135,144

 

TABLE THREE. Associated Signs of Achromatopsia

  Major Signs
  Visual field defect

  Bilateral superior quadrantanopia
  Unilateral superior quadrantanopia


  Topographagnosia
  Prosopagnosia
  Minor Signs
  Object agnosia
  Visual and verbal amnesia
  Pure alexia

 

HEMIACHROMATOPSIA. Unilateral right or left occipital lesions can produce a color defect limited to the contralateral hemifield. The deficit is typically asymptomatic until the defect is demonstrated on a visual examination.145–147 The remaining color vision in the ipsilateral hemifield allows normal or near-normal performance on standard tests of color vision, such as pseudoisochromatic plates and color sorting tasks such as the Farnsworth Munsell 100-Hue Test. Thus, many such cases may go undetected unless the examiner asks the patient to name or sort colors presented in the peripheral field. When hemiachromatopsia is associated with a superior quadrantanopia, the color defect can be demonstrated only in the remaining inferior quadrant.145,146,148

ANATOMY AND PATHOLOGY. Achromatopsia is associated with lesions of the lingual and fusiform gyri, in the ventromedial aspect of the occipital lobe. This was shown pathologically by Verrey in 1888 and MacKay and Dunlop in149 and was confirmed in modern times with CT144,148,150 and MRI.137,141,142 Functional imaging with PET also shows color responses in this region.151 Although early reports suggested a possible right-hemispheric dominance,152 a full-field color defect requires right and left lesions without exception.

The bilateral occipital lesions in achromatopsia are most commonly caused by a stroke, given the common origin of both posterior cerebral arteries from the basilar artery. Bilateral strokes can occur either simultaneously or sequentially. Achromatopsia may be the presenting symptom, or it may evolve from an initial cortical blindness. Other lesions causing achromatopsia are due to herpes simplex encephalitis,142 presumed cerebral metastases,144 repeated focal seizures,153 and focal dementia.154 Transient achromatopsia has also been described in cases of migraine aura.155

THE NATURE OF THE COLOR DEFECT. Normal perceptual color space can be conceived as a threedimensional solid with two chromatic axes and one achromatic axis:

  First chromatic axis (tritan axis): The activity of the short-wavelength-sensitive cone (the blue or S cone) varies, whereas the ratio of the activity of the long-wavelength-sensitive cone (the red or L cone) to that of the medium-wavelength- sensitive cone (the green or M cone) remains constant.
  Second chromatic axis (deutan axis): The ratio of L-cone to M-cone activity varies, whereas S-cone activity remains constant.
  Third (achromatic) axis: The activity of all three cones varies synchronously, such that their activity relative to each other remains constant. The value of light along the achromatic axis is termed brightness. The term luminance is reserved for light sources and reflectance for objects that reflect light.

Conventional color diagrams such as the Committee Internationale de l'Eclairage diagram depict the colors contained within a plane defined by the two chromatic axes. Variations in hue and saturation (the amount to which a color is mixed with white) are contained within such a plane. In cerebral achromatopsia, processing along the two chromatic axes is altered, manifest by abnormal perception of hue and saturation.137 In contrast, discrimination of brightness (tested with sorting of shades of gray) is intact.137,141,156

It should be recognized that color is a property of the appearance of surfaces, and is not inherent to objects.157 Important factors in determining color appearance are the nature of the illuminant (light source), the wavelength composition of the light reflected by the colored object, and the reflectant properties of the scene surrounding the object.158 Color is not equivalent to the wavelengths reflected to the eye. In fact, the colors of objects appear remarkably stable under very different lighting conditions, even though the wavelengths reaching the eye vary widely between those conditions.159 Thus, an apple looks red whether viewed under fluorescent light or sunlight, a phenomenon known as color constancy. To achieve color constancy, it seems likely that the nervous system averages the spectral luminance across large regions of the scene to determine what kind of lighting is present, and then “discounts the illuminant” from the wavelengths reflected by a given object.159,160 Several investigators suggest that area V4 in monkeys may be the site at which color constancy is generated.161–163 Although it has been suggested that human achromatopsia may result from damage to a homologue of monkey V4,149 clinical studies suggest that achromatopsic subjects suffer from a severe defect of hue discrimination at a more elementary level than the generation of color constancy.137,156 Furthermore, lesions of V4 in macaques only mildly impair hue discrimination, if at all.10,164–167 Thus the degree to which the putative human color area involved in achromatopsia can be equated with monkey V4 is unclear.

Not all color information is lost in achromatopsia. Some patients can use chromatic differences to define boundaries between areas of different color but similar brightness,141,142,165 although they do not know what the colors are and cannot order them correctly by hue. This ability may reflect intact opponent color processing in striate cortex.141 Photopic spectral sensitivity curves also have shown preserved trichromacy and color opponency, indicating intact function of the three cones and retinal ganglion cells of the parvocellular pathway.142

TESTING ACHROMATOPSIA. Patients with cerebral achromatopsia affecting the central portions of the visual fields can be tested with the standard procedures used to probe more common disorders such as inherited or acquired achromatopsia of retinal origin. These include anomaloscopic procedures,137,150 in which the subject has to adjust a mixture of color lights (670-nm “red” and 545-nm “green”) to find the unique proportion to match a monochromatic (589-nm “yellow”) target, as on the Nagel Anomaloscope. Tests of color matching and sorting are also useful. The Farnsworth-Munsell 100-Hue Test (Fig. 9) provides a detailed assessment of patients who have failed a screening task such as the Farnsworth D-15. It requires observers to sort isoluminant colors chips along four different axes in color space. The deficits in cerebral achromatopsia affect all hue discriminations,135,137,141,156 unlike common congenital retinal cone defects, although the relative severity of the defect along the red-green and blue-yellow axes may vary.137 Patients with full-field achromatopsia can sometimes discriminate figures on pseudoisochromatic plates. 135,141,142 Zeki149 has argued that performance on pseudoisochromatic plates separates patients into two achromatopsia groups: those with complete achromatopsia and those with incomplete achromatopsia. Heywood and colleagues,142 however, showed that reading of such plates may improve with increased viewing distance, when the individual color dots are no longer resolvable. The ability to read pseudoisochromatic plates at further distances may be due to the retained detection of the chromatic borders of the dots by striate color mechanisms.141,142

Fig. 9. FM 100-Hue Test evaluates just noticeable differences around a hue circle in 85 equidistant steps at isoluminance. Different sectors of the rack are tested with four different racks consisting of 21 to 22 colors each. Plotting the errors produces a graph with a starburst appearance in a patient with severe cerebral chromotopsia.

Testing of color field defects in hemiachromatopsia offers a special challenge because most of the tests described above are designed for viewing in the fovea. In hemiachromatopsia, color vision in at least part of the fovea is spared, so affected patients can achieve normal scores despite having a true color defect.137 Testing color in the peripheral visual field requires (1) monitoring of eye movements to ensure that subjects do not glimpse the target with their fovea or (2) a rapid enough random presentation of targets so that the test is no longer visible if subjects attempt a saccade to the target area. It is also necessary to use larger targets (to compensate for decreased photoreceptor density and concomitant decreased spatial and chromatic discrimination) and to suppress rod activity, which alters color discrimination.

COLOR ANOMIA AND AGNOSIA. Perceiving colors is different from knowing and naming them. Thus achromatopsia must be distinguished from color anomia and agnosia (Table 4). Patients with color anomia or agnosia can still discriminate among different colors, although they have difficulty naming or recognizing them. They do not complain of impaired color perception and may not even be aware that they have a color-naming problem. In contrast, achromatopsics have no particular difficulty producing the name of colors associated with familiar objects, even if they can no longer perceive the differences between colors.

 

TABLE FOUR. Test Results in Various Cerebral Color Defects


  AchromatopsiaDisconnection Color AnomiaDysphasic Color AnomiaColor Agnosia
Color plates* Normal/abnormalNormalNormalNormal
Color sorting AbnormalNormalNormalNormal
Responses to Color Identity    
Question:Response:    
Color shownName colorNormal/abnormalAbnormalAbnormalAbnormal
Color namedPoint to colorNormal/abnormalAbnormalAbnormalAbnormal
Object shownName colorNormalNormalAbnormalAbnormal
Object namedName colorNormal/abnormalNormalAbnormalAbnormal
Object shownPoint to colorNormalNormalNormalAbnormal

*Ishihara.
FM-100 hue.

 

Color anomia is often part of a general anomia in aphasic patients who have no color perception deficits, but it can occur as an isolated entity. Several types of specific color anomia have been described, all in patients with left occipital lesions:

  1. Interhemispheric visual-verbal disconnection syndrome: This is associated with right homonymous hemianopia and alexia without agraphia.168–171 The lesions of patients with this syndrome disrupt callosal connections from the remaining right striate cortex to the language areas of the left hemisphere.169,171 These subjects can see words but cannot read and can see colors but cannot name them. In some, naming of objects is normal,169 suggesting that not all visual information is disconnected.
  2. Color dysphasia170: In addition to having difficulty naming colors of objects they see (visual-verbal task), patients with color dysphasia also have difficulty naming the colors of familiar objects named to them (verbal-verbal task). They cannot complete a sentence like, “A rose is … ”—a task that poses no difficulty for achromatopsic patients.
  3. Short-term color memory deficit: In a study by Davidoff and Ostergaard,172 a patient could not name seen colors but could point accurately to colors named by the examiner. Verbal memory for color names was normal, but visual short-term memory for colors was impaired, which they held to be the primary defect underlying their patient's anomia.

Kinsbourne and Warrington173 described a case of color agnosia, although Oxbury and associates170 considered it color dysphasia. In addition to impaired naming of colors for either visually presented or verbally named objects, their patient could not color line drawings of objects correctly or learn paired associations between seen objects and seen colors (visual-visual task). The defect went beyond anomia because the patient could not associate the appropriate color with objects, despite normal performance on color-matching and color-sorting tasks. The exact site of the lesion was unknown, but the patient also had object anomia and alexia without agraphia.

The Alexias

Acquired alexia is the loss of efficient comprehension of reading material despite adequate visual acuity. Reading is a complex process that involves form perception, spatial attention, scanning saccadic eye movements, and linguistic processing; thus it is not surprising that disturbed reading is a component of many types of cerebral or visual dysfunction. In most of these conditions, alexia is overshadowed by other clinical signs. In most patients, however, impaired reading can also be either the sole or the most dramatic complaint, and these patients may present to the ophthalmologist. The severity of alexia can range from a complete inability to read numbers and individual letters, to mildly slowed reading with occasional errors, confirmed only by comparing reading speed with normal controls matched for educational level.174 The latter type of subtle error is more frequently encountered in developmental dyslexia, a very different condition that Galaburda and co-workers175 and Livingstone and associates176 have attributed to a congenital paucity of neurons in the magnocellular layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus. Table 5 provides a classification of acquired alexic syndromes.

 

TABLE FIVE. Classification of Alexia

  Secondary Alexia
  Alexia due to visual problems

  Hemifield slide
  Hemianopic dyslexia


  Alexia due to attentional problems

  Simultanagnosia
  Neglect dyslexia


  Primary Alexia
  Pure alexia (alexia without agraphia)
  Alexia with agraphia
  “Central” dyslexic syndromes

  Surface dyslexia
  Phonologic dyslexia
  Deep dyslexia


 

The landmark studies of alexia were made by Dejerine.177,178 He first described a patient with alexia and agraphia, but no other linguistic or visual defect, due to a lesion of the left angular gyrus.177 The next year he described a patient with alexia without agraphia (i.e., pure alexia or “word blindness”), associated with an incomplete right homonymous hemianopia, due to a lesion of the left fusiform and lingual gyri.178 This patient subsequently developed agraphia after a second infarct of the left angular gyrus, confirming the findings from the first patient. Dejerine inferred that (1) the left angular gyrus stores the visual representation of words, needed for reading and writing; and (2) alexia without agraphia results if the left angular gyrus is spared, but its visual inputs from both hemispheres are removed (Fig. 10). Dejerine's basic concepts of alexia have survived in Geschwind's28 disconnection theory, and his description of the anatomy of pure alexia remains consistent with the data from modern neuroimaging.179

Fig. 10. Mechanism of pure alexia. Dejerine, in his 1914 textbook, indicated with an X (arrow) the most economical lesion for producing pure alexia. The lesion is located in the white matter near the left occipital horn, where it disrupts the communication of visual information from both the right and left hemispheres from more anterior language-related areas in the left hemisphere.

PURE ALEXIA. The hallmark of this syndrome is a striking dissociation between reading and writing. In extreme cases, patients are unable to read what they have just written. Some alexics can decipher words one letter at a time (i.e., letter-by-letter reading, or spelling dyslexia), whereas others cannot read letters, numbers, words, or other symbols (global dyslexia).180 In patients who use letter-by-letter reading, the time required to read a word characteristically increases with the number of letters in the word.181,182

Pure alexia is frequently associated with a right visual field defect, usually a complete homonymous hemianopia, sometimes a superior quadrantanopia, with or without hemiachromatopsia (Table 6).179 Although field defects can impair reading efficiency, they do not account for pure alexia. Pure alexia can occur without hemianopia,183–186 and many subjects with complete right hemianopia can still read. Other associated defects include color anomia,169,179 anomia for visual objects and photographs, defects of verbal memory, and other types of visual agnosia, including prosopagnosia.179,187

 

TABLE SIX. Associated Defects of Pure Alexia

  Major Signs
  Visual field defect

  Right homonymous hemianopia
  Right superior quadrantanopia


  Right hemiachromatopsia
  Color anomia
  Minor Signs
  Visual object anomia
  Verbal amnesia

 

Covert Reading in Pure Alexia. Several studies have suggested that some covert reading ability is retained in some patients with pure alexia, analogous to the covert abilities in blindsight and prosopagnosia. Some patients with pure alexia can indicate whether a string of letters forms a word (lexical decision task).182,188,189 Another patient was able to indicate which letters in a string of letters formed a word and also could point to a written word named by the examiner, even though she was unable to read the word aloud.190 In another study, a patient was able to identify rapidly presented letters better if the letters were part of words than if they were parts of random letter strings.181 Not all patients with pure alexia, however, possess covert abilities.191,192 As with blindsight, the factors that determine whether such covert reading ability is retained by a given patient with pure alexia remain to be elucidated.

Anatomy and Mechanisms of Pure Alexia. Lesions causing pure alexia are most commonly located in the medial and inferior occipitotemporal region.179,180 The most commonly invoked explanatory mechanism of pure alexia remains the visual-verbal disconnection proposed by Dejerine178 and Geschwind.28 Callosal pathways transmitting visual information from visual association cortex of the right hemisphere to language centers in the left hemisphere are interrupted by a lesion in the splenium, forceps major, or paraventricular white matter surrounding the occipital horn of the lateral ventricle.179 Visual information from the right hemifield is either absent in cases with right hemianopia, or also interrupted in its course to the left language centers. In fact, right hemianopia from lesions elsewhere in the visual pathways, such as the left geniculate nucleus, can lead to pure alexia when combined with a splenial lesion.193,194 Cases of pure alexia without hemianopia have been found in patients with lesions of the white matter underlying the angular gyrus. Such “subangular” lesions presumably interrupt the input to angular gyrus from both hemispheres at a very distal site.183–185 Although some cases of pure alexia without hemianopia suggest that the callosal fibers from the right hemisphere travel in white matter tracts inferior to the occipital horn,184,185 Binder and Mohr180 proposed that some fibers may travel dorsal to the occipital horn. They also hypothesized that preservation of these dorsal callosal fibers is critical to retained letter-by-letter reading in spelling dyslexia, which they consider a partial form of pure alexia, compared with global pure alexia, which they consider the complete form.

Although disconnection theory dominates the discussion of pure alexia, other interpretations of the defect also exist. Some argue that it represents a form of simultanagnosia (see Balint's syndrome).101 Others have argued that letter-by-letter readers are unable to access information about the visual word-form: that is, pure alexia may be a specific type of visual agnosia.191,195 In keeping with this, a recent investigation found that a patient with pure alexia had difficulty perceiving complex textures, suggesting a problem with local pattern analysis.132 It was suggested that alexia and prosopagnosia are hallmarks of two different types of visual agnosia, stemming from basic differences in right and left hemispheric visual processing.

HEMIALEXIAS. The disconnection hypothesis advanced by Dejerine178 and Geschwind28 involves two deafferentions of the left hemisphere reading process: the disconnection of right-hemisphere visual information and the disconnection or destruction of left-hemisphere visual information. Each of these two disconnections have been described in isolation, both causing hemialexias. Greenblatt196 reported left hemialexia, in which reading was impaired in the left but not the right visual hemifield because of isolated damage to callosal fibers. A similar left hemialexia follows surgical section of the posterior corpus callosum.197 Right hemialexia due to a lesion of the left medial and ventral occipital lobe that spared other visual functions in the right field also has been reported.198 Left hemiparalexia is yet another rare disconnection syndrome attributed to splenial damage.199 The reading pattern in this disorder is similar to that in left hemineglect dyslexia (see later in chapter) in that substitution and omission errors occur for the first letter of words. Patients with left hemiparalexia, however, do not have any evidence of hemineglect and have right rather than left hemianopia.

ALEXIA WITH AGRAPHIA, LETTER BLINDNESS, AND CENTRAL DYSLEXIAS. Alexia with agraphia is characterized by relatively intact oral and auditory linguistic function, but deficits in reading and writing skills. Dejerine177,178 and others200 showed that lesions of the left angular gyrus caused alexia with agraphia, although lesions of the temporoparietal junction also have been implicated.201 In Gerstmann's syndrome, alexia with agraphia is also accompanied by acalculia, right-left disorientation, and finger agnosia.

Another form of alexia and agraphia is described as an accompaniment of Broca's aphasia (nonfluent aphasia), due to lesions of the left inferior frontal operculum.202,203 The difficulty these patients have with reading aloud and writing is understandable given their nonfluent aphasia; however, their ability to comprehend written material is also impaired. These patients are better at occasionally grasping a whole word, without being able to name the letters of the word. For this reason it has sometimes been called “literal alexia” or “letter blindness,” as opposed to word blindness in pure alexia. These patients also have impaired comprehension of syntactic structure of sentences (grammar), just as their speech output often demonstrates agrammatism.

Central dyslexias are unusual reading problems that have been described more recently. Many of these occur in association with other aphasic features, but they can occur as isolated dyslexias also. They are labeled central dyslexias because they reflect dysfunction of central reading processes rather than “peripheral” attentional or visual processes. Central dyslexias are formulated in terms of reading models derived from cognitive neuropsychology. A review of central dyslexia is beyond the scope of this chapter, but can be found in Black and Behrmann.174

SECONDARY ALEXIA: VISION AND ATTENTION. Patients with normal acuity but visual field defects can have reading problems secondary to visual loss. Complete bitemporal hemianopia from lesions affecting the optic chiasm can cause “hemifield slide,” in which the absence of overlapping regions of visual field between the two eyes leads to a breakdown of binocular alignment.204 This in turn causes episodic horizontal deviation, which is manifest by transient diplopia or disappearance of objects along the vertical meridian, and episodic vertical deviation, which is manifest by a vertical step in objects crossing the vertical meridian. Thus, when reading, letters may disappear or duplicate and lines of words may be confused as they cross from one hemifield into the other (Fig. 11). Patients with complete homonymous hemianopia may also complain of reading problems. With languages written from left to right, patients with left hemianopia may have difficulty returning to the beginning of the next line because the left margin disappears into their scotoma as they scan rightward. Marking their place with an L-shaped ruler may reduce this frustration, but may be futile if hemianopia is compounded by left hemineglect.

Fig. 11. Hemifield slide—effects on reading. In patients with bitemporal hemianopia, episodes of binocular misalignment can occur in both vertical and horizontal directions. These misalignments between the nasal hemifield of the eyes are illustrated on the right side of the diagram. The effects of such misalignments on the reading of a paragraph are shown on the left side. The result is a type of “secondary alexia,” that differs from the cerebral mechanism for pure alexia illustrated by Dejerine.

Attentional problems can also affect reading. In attentional dyslexia, the perception of single items is adequate, but perception of several objects simultaneously is impaired.205,206 These patients can identify single words normally but have difficulty with several words together; similarly, they can identify single letters but cannot name the letters in a visually presented word. They may demonstrate literal migration errors, in which a letter from another word is substituted at the same place in another word (e.g., BONE TURN becomes TONE TURN). Letters may be mistaken for others similar in appearance (e.g., O for C, but not T for C).206 Attentional dyslexia has been associated with lesions in the left parietal lobe205 or temporo-occipital junction.206

Neglect dyslexia is another attentional reading problem, most often associated with right parietal lobe lesions. The hemineglect for the left side of space is reflected in left-sided reading errors.207 Such patients may omit reading the left side of a line, of a page, or of individual words (Fig. 12). With the latter, they may make omissions (e.g., PLANE becomes LANE), additions (e.g., ART becomes CART) or substitutions (e.g., TURN becomes BURN). These defects appear specific for the left side of space rather than the beginning of words because such errors do not occur for vertically printed words.207

Fig. 12. Neglect alexia. The eye movements (black lines) and the fixations (dots) during paragraph reading are shown in a patient with left hemineglect. This patient failed to read words on the left side of a block of text (Behrmann, Black, and Barton, unpublished data). Such a problem is most often caused by a right parietal lobe lesion. The failure to attend to stimuli on the left is generally not restricted to reading or even to vision.

DORSAL PATHWAY

“Balint's Syndrome” and Associated Visuospatial Disorders

In 1909, Balint208 reported an unusual triad of visual behavior defects in a man with bilateral cerebrovascular lesions:

  1. “Spatial disorder of attention”: Wolpert209 later called this simultanagnosia and defined it as “an inability to interpret the totality of a picture scene despite preservation of ability to apprehend individual portions of the whole.”
  2. Optic ataxia”: A defect of hand movements under visual guidance despite normal limb strength and position sense.
  3. “Psychic paralysis of gaze”: Also known as “spasm of fixation”or ocular apraxia,4 this is defined as the inability to move the eyes voluntarily despite unrestricted ocular rotations.

The visual fields in Balint's patient were reportedly normal for white objects and colors, and visual acuity was 20/40 or better; however, the man's attention appeared to quickly wane, producing a “concentric constriction” of the useful field of view. Balint believed his subject could see only one object at a time, no matter what its size. He reported that the man's focal point of attention was displaced 35° to 40° into the right hemifield and that he would read the letters only at the right ends of lines of text, a finding compatible with a left hemineglect syndrome (see Fig. 12). Defective reaching was worse with the right hand, and Balint believed both hands should have been affected equally if the problem was just visual. Also, when Balint positioned the left hand, the man could imitate the same position with his right hand, showing that position sense was normal. The patient died of cerebrovascular complications, and an autopsy was performed (Fig. 13). Balint emphasized that the subject had bilateral lesions of the angular gyri and that the calcarine cortex was preserved; however, there was also extensive damage to other visual areas and connections, including damage to the posterior corpus callosum and white matter bilaterally as well as the pulvinar of the thalamus.

Fig. 13. Balint (1909) reported on a man with a “spatial disorder of attention” (simultanagnosia), “psychic paralysis of gaze” (ocular apraxia), and optic ataxia (see text). The onset occurred in 1894, the patient died in 1906, and an autopsy was performed. Balint pictured the lesions in his patient on lateral views of the hemispheres. He emphasized the role of bilateral lesions of the angular gyri and the sparing of the primary visual cortex (Brodmann's area 17). However, the patient also had damage to the posterior white matter, in which optic radiations travel on both sides, and to the pulvinar, a critical subcortical structure for visuospatial integration.

Holmes's 1918 report210 on disturbances of visual orientation is often compared with Balint's. Holmes studied six soldiers with missile wounds of the occiput causing “an affection of the power of localizing the position in space and the distance of objects by sight alone.” The men had “moderately good” visual acuity and stereoacuity, and Holmes doubted that their various visual field defects (including homonymous hemianopia, quadrantanopia, paracentral scotomata, and extensive loss of peripheral vision) could account for their visual disorientation (i.e., errors in judging the relative location and distance of objects). The soldiers failed to detect objects presented in their “normal” visual fields if they were otherwise occupied, but Holmes distinguished this from “the unilateral ‘disturbance of visual attention;” that occurs “as a result of parietal and lateral occipital lesions.” He believed that his subjects' difficulty counting coins and navigating were due to multiple defects, including the spatial localization defect, failure to attend to objects in the periphery, and defective fixation and search. Altogether, their abilities “contrasted unfavorably with a blind man.” Two soldiers (cases 2 and 5) died, and autopsies were performed. The soldier in case 2 had an entry wound in the lateral surface of the right occipital lobe, with exit in the area of the left angular gyrus affecting the “dorsal portions of the sagittal bundles” in both hemispheres, “posterior to the splenium and dorsal to the calcarine fissures,” in the region of the parieto-occipital sulci. In case 5, the soldier had an entry wound in the left hemisphere destroying the supramarginal gyrus, with softening toward the central fissure. The missile tract lay above the splenium, and the exit wound affected the right angular gyrus. Craniometric assessments of the entry and exit wounds in the skulls of the survivors also suggested similar bilateral dorsolateral lesions.

Hécaen and Ajuriaguerra210a are credited with coining the term Balint's syndrome. They reported on four patients with “minor forms of Balint's syndrome” who showed sluggish exploration of the visual environment, which they attributed to “sticky fixation,” or restriction of fixation to a single point. They also recognized a “motor component” of their patients' defects, relating it to possible damage in posterior parietal and occipital association cortices concerned with ocular pursuit. Precise anatomic localization was not possible, as in most cases that antedate the era of modern neuroimaging procedures. Luria211 reported on “disorders of simultaneous perception” in a Polish officer, shot in the head in the waning days of World War II. The brain damage resulted in “vestibular giddiness,” “fits,” and severe constriction of the visual fields on a perimetric examination. The patient could perceive objects “in an essentially normal manner,” but “moved his eyes from one object to another in a confused and inadequate manner” leading to “failure to fixate one or more of the objects present.” The patient tended to neglect objects on the left, as did the patients of Balint, Holmes, and Hécaen and Ajuriaguerra. Luria's212 subject also showed defective hand movements under visual guidance, and “his gaze manifest the same helplessness as his arm.” He believed that his patient had “limitation of visual attention,” “incapacity to combine details into a coherent whole,” and “piecemeal perception” due to a “weakness in cortical tonus” within the visual cortex causing each “focus of excitation” within that area to “inhibit the remainder of the visual cortex by negative induction.”

There have been several objections to the need for the designation “Balint's syndrome.213” The full syndrome is generally associated with a wide variety of behavioral disturbances, which raises questions as to the existence of a specific underlying mechanism. These disturbances include cortical blindness, Anton's syndrome (denial of cortical blindness), bilateral visual field defects including central or paracentral scotomata, bilateral upper quadrantanopia, metamorphopsia, palinopsia, monocular polyopia, poor visual acuity, cerebral akinetopsia, and dementia (Table 7).213 Many aspects of the disorder are indistinguishable from the hemineglect (right parietal lobe) syndrome, violating the principle of autonomy for a syndrome.

 

TABLE SEVEN. Balint's Syndrome and Other Disorders of Visuospatial Processing

  Balint's syndrome

  Simultanagnosia (visual disorientation)
  Optic ataxia
  Ocular apraxia


  Left hemineglect (hemiattention) syndrome
  Cerebral akinetopsia (defective motion perception)

  Inability to perceive a directional motion signal
  Reduced dynamic visual acuity
  Inability to perceive 2-D and 3-D structure from motion cues
  Defective flicker perception


  Astereopsis (static or dynamic)
  Palinopsia (persistence of visual afterimages)
  Upside-down vision
  Metamorphopsia (carnival mirror-like effect)*
  Monocular polyopia (multiple ghostly images of a single object)*
  Micropsia or macropsia (objects looking smaller or larger, respectively, than they should)*


*Not caused by distortions of the ocular media or surface, and not improved by looking through a pinhole.

 

Indeed, each of Balint's original triad of defects, particularly simultanagnosia, may represent relatively broad categories that subsume other combinations of defects of widely different degrees of severity. Moreover, the main proposed mechanism, an inability to see more than one object at a time, no matter what its size, is not sound. The first half of this assertion was an exaggeration. True, Balint's patient performed worse in the detection, description, or identification of visual stimuli when several were present, which is compatible with constriction of the fields of attention. Yet it is clear from Balint's own observations that his subject could process more than a single object at once, as when he successfully read whole sections of text (comprising several words). The second half of Balint's assertion, that his subject could see any single object, no matter what its size, is erroneous. This claim was based on the patient's ability to recognize relatively large targets, such as a person, by vision alone. Yet, the recognition of persons or their faces can be accomplished with economy, based on just a few features. Normal subjects can recognize degraded representations of persons as in caricatures of famous faces, or they can identify familiar persons from glimpses, silhouettes, or partial views. Recognizing a person or object, as Balint claimed his subject could do, can never prove that all of the object is being seen simultaneously. (See Ball and colleagues214 for more modern insights on reductions in the the useful field of view in conditions of attentional load or impairment.)

Nevertheless, Balint's report is valuable because it suggested that the dorsolateral visual association cortices are critical for processing visuospatial information and for visual attention.213 It also provided preliminary clues as to the neural substrates of visually guided reaching and grasping behavior. Subsequent studies have supported the presence of attention-related neurons in the visual association cortex of primates and humans.215–217 These later studies have also confirmed the role of structures in the parietal lobe and occipitoparietal regions, including Brodmann's areas 5, 7, superior 19, 37, and 39 in the neural control of visually guided reaching and grasping of objects in extrapersonal space (Fig. 14).218 Damage in these areas is now also reported to impair motion perception219 (see Disorders of Motion Perception).

Fig. 14. Reaching and pointing to visual targets in a case of optic ataxia are shown. The patient is a 65-year-old man with severe vertebrobasilar vascular disease. First he had a left occipital lobe infarction causing a right homonymous hemianopia. Next he had a large infarction of the right parietal lobe, which included the angular gyrus. Subsequently, his behavior resembled that of Balint's patient. To demonstrate optic ataxia, the patient is viewed from above as he reaches for highly visible targets located at arm's length to the left and right of body midline. The recordings use an optoelectronic technique to track an infrared-emitting diode fastened to the dorsum of each index fingertip. Multiple reaches are shown with the left hand and right hand. All movements begin from the same midline position just anterior to the patient. Head movement is unrestricted. Notice that the paths of the hand movements are highly variable. Also, the end positions of the movements are highly inaccurate, especially to the left target. The patient's hand movements were improved for reaches to self-bound targets such as his own nose (not shown), a task that depends more on kinesthetic than visual guidance. (Rizzo M: Balint's syndrome and associated visuospatial disorders. In Kennard C (ed): Balliere's International Practice and Research, pp 415–437. Philadelphia, WB Saunders, 1993)

The many causes of “Balint's syndrome” and related disorders of visuospatial processing include cerebrovascular disease (especially watershed infarctions),8,220,221 tumor, trauma, “slow virus” or prion infections such as Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, HIV infections,222 and degenerative conditions such as Alzheimer's disease.223,224

In these cases, as in the other disorders of cerebral integration outlined in this chapter, a basic ophthalmologic examination is crucial to rule out disorders of the retina or ocular media. Perimetry may be difficult because of attentional difficulties, fixational lapses, and easy fatigue. The use of neuropsychologic probes, as summarized by Tranel,225 is most helpful.

Disorders of Motion Perception

MOTION PERCEPTION IN NONHUMAN PRIMATES. Initial evidence for motion-sensitivity in extrastriate visual cortex came from studies of monkeys. These studies have revealed several motion-sensitive regions. Neurons in the MT area of the superior temporal sulcus were first found to be selective for the direction and speed of motion,226–230 but not for shape or color.227,230,231 MT projects to the adjacent MST and ventral intraparietal (VIP) areas.232 MST has two distinct regions: a ventral portion responds best to the relative motion of small objects, and a dorsal portion responds best to motion over large regions of the visual field.233 Dorsal MST also has neurons that respond to large complex motion patterns, such as expanding or contracting radial patterns and rotating motion.234–237 The VIP area has not been extensively investigated, but it is reported to contain cells that discharge in the presence of a moving object that would collide with the monkey's face.238

The anterior superior temporal polysensory (STPa) area contains cells responsive to form239 and others responsive to motion.240 Some STPa cells discharge selectively for complex body movements, such as hand actions, gait, and head movement,239,241 and may represent an integration of visual input from both dorsal and ventral pathways. Some regions in the ventral (form) pathway also respond to moving stimuli. For example, form- and orientation-selective neurons in the IT cortex respond to their preferred stimulus regardless of whether the defining visual attribute is luminance, texture, or motion.242,243

Considering that there are already direction-selective neurons in striate cortex, several studies have asked what specific contribution MT makes to motion perception. Experiments with transparent motion244–246 and moving plaids247,248 show that some MT neurons may reduce motion noise by averaging motion vectors over large regions to derive a judgment of common global motion. In contrast, V1 neurons respond whenever motion in their preferred direction is present, without regard for the presence of other moving stimuli. Noise reduction within MT is restricted to motion at the same stereoscopic depth, while motion at different depths and presumably from different objects continues to be represented, allowing the perception of transparent moving surfaces.249

Motion can also be differentiated into first- and second-order types. First-order motion involves displacement of luminant object borders, whereas second-order motion is defined solely by movement of features such as texture and stereo disparity, without changes in mean luminance. Most MT cells respond to both first- and second-order motion, suggesting that MT functions as a general motion detector.229,250 Another possible role of some MT neurons is the analysis of relative motion. The direction-selective responses of some MT neurons are inhibited by motion in a similar direction in the surrounding visual field.251–253 This surround inhibition may play a role in figure-ground discrimination, perceptual constancy, and motion cues to depth perception. It may also aid in accurate judgment of object trajectory during eye movements by comparing object motion to the movement of the background across the retina as the eye moves.

Lesions of MT or MST cause pursuit and saccades to targets in areas of the contralateral visual field to underestimate target velocity (retinotopic motion defects).254–257 This is thought to represent abnormal motion analysis rather than an oculomotor defect because the defect is restricted to a contralateral area of visual space, is specific for moving targets, and affects both pursuit and saccades. Deficient motion perception in the contralateral hemifield has been confirmed with random dot cinematograms that require subjects to judge motion direction from a background of motion noise.258 Schiller10 also found mild-to-moderate contralateral retinotopic impairments in motion detection, motion direction discrimination, and speed discrimination, though all defects were less severe than those caused by geniculate lesions, suggesting that other cortical areas participate in motion processing. Recovery of pursuit and perceptual defects after small MT or MST lesions usually occurs within 2 weeks and likely depends on surviving portions of MT or MST.254,255,258,259 Recovery after complete lesions of both MT and MST also occurs but is slower and incomplete and likely involves other cortical areas.259 Large bilateral lesions of MT and MST create enduring impairments in a range of motion tasks.260,261

Lesions of either monkey MT/MST261 or IT cortex262 impair perception of motion-defined form, implying that elements of both the ventral and dorsal streams participate in the perception of form-from-motion. In support of this concept, Schiller10 found that lesions of either V4 or MT alone produced only mild defects in the discrimination of rectangular shapes defined by moving dots against a background of stationary dots, but that concurrent lesions caused a moderate defect.

In monkeys viewing random-dot cinematograms, microstimulation in the MT area was found to affect judgment of direction.263–265 Small currents biased the decision in the preferred direction of the stimulated neurons, whereas large currents impaired discrimination, presumably by increasing motion noise from current spread into adjacent neuronal columns.

Psychophysical and physiologic performance have been assessed simultaneously in monkeys viewing coherence-based random dot cinematograms.266 “Neurometric” thresholds based on the firing rates of MT neurons were similar to the psychophysical performance of the monkey for direction discrimination. In a similar experiment with MST neurons, Celebrini and Newsome267 found similar results, and showed that neuronal and psychophysical thresholds were both affected by changes in stimulus size and speed. Thus it is possible that motion direction discrimination in monkeys is supported sufficiently by a small number of neurons in either MT or MST.

TESTING MOTION PERCEPTION. Motion perception can play many roles in vision.268 One role is the perception of moving objects in the environment. Since such objects usually occupy only a small portion of the visual field, one can obtain a sense of their relative motion by comparing their motion with that of the background. Perception of object motion is used to guide reaching movements of the limbs and tracking eye movements, such as saccades and smooth pursuit. Information about the movement of the observer can also be derived from motion perception. As the observer moves or turns his or her head or eyes, the image of the entire visual environment moves in the opposite direction. Thus, motion occurring over large portions of the visual field usually implies that the observer rather than an external object is moving. This large field of motion generates optokinetic responses that complement the vestibulo-ocular reflex in stabilizing the line of sight during head- or self-motion. It is also possible to derive information about object identity from visual motion: The difference in motion between a figure and its background aids in segmenting its two-dimensional form, and the pattern of velocity gradients within an object encodes the object's three-dimensional form.

Tests for these different aspects of motion perception exist. Studies of eye movements have long revealed abnormalities of smooth pursuit and optokinetic nystagmus with cerebral lesions, but whether these represent motor or perceptual defects cannot be determined definitively from eye movements alone. Animated displays with random dots or moving gratings have come into use more recently. Displays can be designed to probe whether subjects can discriminate motion direction, motion speed, the presence of a motion boundary, and forms defined by motion (Fig. 15). Most of these are still experimental.

Fig. 15. Random-Dot Tests of Motion Perception. In each of these four diagrams the length and direction of arrows represent the speed and direction of a moving dot, and dotted lines are imaginary. Motion boundaries are detected when two regions with differing motion are seen as distinct. Perception of global motion direction is tested with a display containing a mixture of coherently moving signal dots (black arrows) and randomly moving noise dots (white arrows). For three-dimensional structure, a hollow sphere is perceived from the distribution of dots moving at different speeds across its apparent surface. Two-dimensional structure requires detection of motion boundaries between the object and its background.

HUMAN CEREBRAL AKINETOPSIA. Lesions of extrastriate cortex can produce a defect in motion perception, which Zeki269 called cerebral akinetopsia. The cases of two such patients, who had bilateral lesions of lateral temporo-occipital cortex, have been well-described in the literature. The most well known is the case of L.M., a patient who suffered a sagittal sinus thrombosis with cerebral infarction of lateral aspects of Brodmann's areas 18, 19, and 39 bilaterally.219,270–273 Patient A.F. had bilateral lesions of lateral occipitotemporal cortex caused by an acute hypertensive hemorrhage.195 In both patients, spatial and temporal aspects of contrast sensitivity, shape, orientation, and color discrimination, as well as complex tasks such as matching forms and recognizing faces and objects, were relatively normal. In L.M., detection of motion was relatively preserved both centrally and in the peripheral field for moderate velocities (up to 8°/sec), but greater impairments were evident on more complex motion tasks. Her ability to predict the trajectory of moving objects and her ocular smooth pursuit were abnormal, and there was dramatic impairment of discrimination of temporal frequency and drift velocity of gratings. Small amounts of random motion or even stationary noise degraded her discrimination of motion direction and identification of three-dimensional shapes from motion cues. Similarly, A.F. was severely impaired in discriminating speed differences, direction in the presence of motion noise, and two-dimensional forms on the basis of regions with relative differences in motion speed.

Some impairments on nonmotion tasks were also evident in these two patients. L.M. also could not perceive form on the basis of texture, dynamic stereo, and static density cues. A.F. was poor at recognizing objects from unusual viewpoints and in incomplete outline drawings. The patient also showed impairment on tests of spatial vision (e.g., hyperacuity, line orientation, line bisection, spatial location) and stereopsis.

Unilateral lesions of extrastriate cortex cause more subtle abnormalities of motion perception. Motion defects restricted to the contralateral hemifield have been described. These include speed discrimination,274,275 detecting boundaries between regions with different motion,195 and discriminating direction from backgrounds of motion noise.276 In these studies, motion detection and contrast thresholds for motion direction274,275 were normal. Lesions were located in the lateral occipitotemporal cortex or in the inferior parietal lobule. As with hemiachromatopsia, such hemiakinetopic defects may not be common because hemianopia caused by damage to optic radiations or striate cortex frequently accompanies natural lateral occipitotemporal lesions.

Abnormalities demonstrated by centrally viewed motion tests also can occur in patients with unilateral lesions, circumventing the problem of hemianopia. Vaina277 found that patients with right occipitotemporal lesions could not identify two-dimensional structures from motion cues, whereas patients with right occipitoparietal lesions could not discriminate velocity or detect three-dimensional structures from motion cues. Regan and associates278 reported difficulty in detecting or recognizing motion-defined letters in patients with white-matter lesions underlying the lateral temporo-occipital cortex of either hemisphere. Nawrot and colleagues279 found abnormal direction discrimination in seven subjects with right hemisphere lesions. Five had a lesion in the occipitotemperoparietal area, and two had other lesions: a ventromedial occipital in one and an insular lesion in the other. Impaired direction discrimination of motion toward the side of the lesion has been found in cases of lesions of the lateral occipitotemporal area.276

These lesion studies suggest that, in humans, a variety of complex motion tasks involve the lateral occipitotemporal or occipitoparietal cortex. These tasks include discrimination of the speed of motion vectors, integration of vectors for discrimination of global direction and three-dimensional structure, and segregation of motion vectors to discriminate the two-dimensional structure of shapes and letters. Simpler tasks of motion perception, however (e.g., detection of motion, discrimination of motion in the absence of noise), are not impaired. This implies that more elementary motion signals are processed at other, possibly earlier, cortical sites. The role of lateral occipitotemporal cortex may be to use these simpler motion signals to derive a higher order representation of motion. Therefore its destruction may not leave a patient truly motion-blind, but impaired at complex analyses of motion. Furthermore, other aspects of spatial analysis may be impaired,195,270 as might be expected of a lesion affecting the dorsal pathway.

Back to Top
POSITIVE PHENOMENA
Up to now the discussion on the effects of cerebral lesions on vision has concentrated on lost capacities, or negative phenomena. On occasion, however, lesions of visual cortex may create positive phenomena when abnormal visual images are seen by the patient. These abnormal visual images can be classified into two main types: visual perseverations and visual hallucinations.

VISUAL PERSEVERATION

The persistence, recurrence, or duplication of a visual image is a rare complaint. The term palinopsia (or paliopsia) refers to the perseveration of a visual image in time.280 It is distinguished from visual hallucinations by the fact that the palinopsic illusion is determined by the elements of a previously or currently viewed scene, whereas the content of visual hallucinations lacks a relation to current or recent visual experience. Nevertheless, the distinction is sometimes difficult, and some patients have both perseverative and hallucinatory phenomena.280,281 Perseveration of a visual image in space has been termed cerebral diplopia or cerebral polyopia: these phenomena describe the perception of one or more duplicates of a seen object simultaneously.282,283 Another type of spatial perseveration occurs when the contents or surface of an object appear to spread beyond the true spatial boundaries of the object, a phenomenon termed illusory visual spread.280 Thus a wallpaper pattern may appear to spread beyond the true borders of the wall, and the pattern of a garment may spread over the wearer's face.280 Some patients experience perseveration in both space and time, resulting for example in palinopsic polyopia.283 A form of this occurs in patients who report that as an object moves they see multiple copies of the object in its trail, although this may not be true polyopia. Bender and associates281 argued that because palinopsic images were often larger than the original images, this represented an illusory visual spread concomitant with palinopsia; however, Critchley280 considered this combination exceptional.

Palinopsia, the abnormal persistence of a visual image in time, may take two forms:

  Immediate type: An image continues to be seen long after the actual scene has disappeared, eventually fading after a period of several minutes usually. This type of palinopsia thus has some similarity to the normal afterimage experienced after prolonged viewing of a bright object. Indeed, detailed investigations of some palinopsic patients have suggested that the differences between normal afterimages and palinopsic images are primarily quantitative284; however, other studies disagree.281
  Delayed type: An image of a previously seen object may reappear after an interval of minutes to hours, sometimes repeatedly for days or even weeks.284 Some patients have both immediate and delayed types of palinopsia.284

The location of the perseverated image in the visual field can vary. In some patients, it persists in the same location as the original image (usually foveal) and moves as the eyes move, much like a normal afterimage.285 In other patients, the image reappears in a coexistent visual field defect280; indeed, Bender and colleagues281 considered palinopsia to be a transient feature in the evolution of homonymous field defects. In other patients, the perseverated image is multiplied across otherwise intact visual fields.283 The location of palinopsic images is sometimes quite contextually specific. For example, some patients report that after they view a face on television, everyone else in the room appears to have the same face as the person on television; others report that the print on a sign over one shop reappears on the signs over other shops.280,283,286 Although some of these visual descriptions may represent a sophisticated form of palinopsic polyopia, others may actually be consistent with a constant foveal or perifoveal location of a perseverative image that manifests itself repeatedly when the context is appropriate.

Visual perseveration can be accompanied by a wide variety of other symptoms. Almost always there is an associated homonymous visual field defect. Both upper92,286 and lower284 quadrantanopias have been reported. Other illusions of spatial aspects of stimuli can accompany visual perseveration, such as metamorphopsia (the distortion of objects), macropsia (the illusion that objects are larger than in reality), and micropsia (the illusion that objects are smaller than in reality).280,281,284,287 Less frequently, signs of a “ventral stream” lesion have been reported, such as topographagnosia, prosopagnosia, and achromatopsia.92,281 Visual perseveration within other sensory modalities may also occur: Cummings and co-workers285 reported on a patient with both visual and somatosensory perseveration.

The prognosis for visual perseveration varies. Bender and associates281 considered palinopsia to be a rare but transient phase in the resolution or progression of a visual field defect, usually lasting days to months. Others, however, have described palinopsia persisting for months to years.284,285,288,289 Anticonvulsant treatment may help some types of palinopsia,288 but not others.285

Mechanisms

The pathophysiology of palinopsia remains obscure. Four main hypotheses have been advanced, as summarized by Bender and colleagues.281 These include a pathologic exaggeration of the normal afterimage, a seizure disorder, hallucinations, and psychogenic origin.

EXAGGERATION OF THE NORMAL AFTERIMAGE. This hypothesis was suggested by Kinsbourne and Warrington's study284 of two patients with shared palinopsic features. These features included a dependence of the prominence of the palinopsic image on the intensity and duration of the initial stimulus, a negative (i.e., complementary color) afterimage when visualized against a light background, a binocular afterimage after monocular viewing of an object, and motion of the afterimage in the same direction as active eye movements but in the opposite direction when the eyes are moved passively. However, not all cases of immediate palinopsia exhibit these features.281 In particular, the color of palinopsic images tends not to depend strongly on the colors of the original stimulus.281,289

SEIZURE DISORDER. The hypothesis that palinopsia is a manifestation of epilepsy is supported circumstantially by the fact that several patients in early series280,281 had other features that suggested seizures, such as episodic loss of consciousness, tongue biting, and confusion. Epileptiform abnormalities seen on electroencephalograms and improvement of palinopsia with anticonvulsants have been reported in some cases of patients with the immediate type of palinopsia,287,288 although it should be stressed that improvement with anticonvulsants does not prove ictal origin.

HALLUCINATIONS. The third hypothesis is that palinopsia may be a nonictal hallucinatory state. Some patients with palinopsia have coexistent nonpalinopsic visual hallucinations, and the location of perseverative images within visual field defects is reminiscent of the release hallucinations of the Charles Bonnet syndrome. Hallucinogenic drugs have been known to cause palinopsic illusions as well.284 Cummings and co-workers285 suggested that delayed palinopsia with concurrent hemianopia was a release hallucination, whereas immediate palinopsia was more likely ictal in origin, especially in the absence of a visual field defect. The value of such a distinction remains to be proved.

PSYCHOGENIC DISORDER. The possibility of a psychogenic basis was considered by Critchley,280 who postulated that palinopsic reports may be a confabulatory response to other visual dysfunction.

Anatomy and Pathology

In investigating visual perseveration, intoxication with hallucinogens such as mescaline and lysergide (LSD) must be excluded. There are also isolated reports of palinopsia and other visual illusions caused by prescribed medication, such as clomiphene,290 interleukin 2,291 and trazodone.292 Rarely, palinopsia occurs in psychiatric conditions such as schizophrenia293 and psychotic depression,294 but as with visual hallucinations in such patients, the visual symptom is always accompanied by other signs of mental illness.

Once these conditions are excluded, however, visual perseveration virtually always indicates a cerebral lesion. One possible exception was a case of immediate palinopsia whose only known visual pathway lesions were bilateral optic neuritis295; however, this patient had clinically definite multiple sclerosis with prior brainstem symptoms, so without MRI one cannot exclude subclinical cerebral hemispheric lesions. The value of palinopsia in localizing cerebral lesions more precisely, however, is uncertain. Bender and associates281 found a predominance of right parieto-occipital lesions, although they stated that left hemispheric lesions may have been underrepresented because of coexisting aphasia. Critchley280 documented right, left, and bilateral lesions with palinopsia. Bender's parieto-occipital location is contested by the demonstration of medial occipital and occipitotemporal lesions in some patients' CT and autopsy specimens.283,286 In a rare case, no lesion was found with neuroimaging.289

VISUAL HALLUCINATIONS

Hallucinations are defined as perceptions without external stimulation of the relevant sensory organ. They are not uncommon in patients with abnormal mental states due to dementia or confusional states secondary to metabolic or toxic insults. Visual hallucinations also occur in cases of psychiatric illness, but they usually are accompanied by other signs of mental illness and hallucinations in other sensory modalities, especially auditory. In persons with intact cognition and mental function, however, isolated visual hallucinations can occur. In these cases, the hallucinations are often a sign of underlying neurologic or ophthalmologic disease.

Isolated visual hallucinations can be categorized into three pathophysiologic groups: “release” hallucinations, visual seizures, and migraine.

“Release” Hallucinations (Charles Bonnet Syndrome)

It is increasingly recognized that visual loss of any origin can result in visual hallucinations. These are called release hallucinations because it is thought that they somehow arise or are released in visual cortex by the absence of incoming sensory impulses. Patients with release hallucinations are characterized as having a clear mental state, associated central or peripheral visual field defects (usually binocular), awareness that the visions are not real, lack of distress, and lack of hallucinations in other sensory modalities. One series reported release hallucinations occurring in up to 57% of patients with a variety of causes of visual loss.296 Hallucinations are often classified as simple or complex: Unformed lights, colors, and shapes are seen in the former; recognizable objects and figures are seen in the latter. Lepore296 estimates that the simple type of hallucination is at least twice as common as the complex type. Several series have suggested an incidence of 10% to 30% for complex hallucinations in visual loss,296–299 so even these more dramatic forms are not unusual. It has been suggested that the true incidence of these is often underestimated because of a reluctance on the part of patients to mention hallucinations, for fear of being labeled “crazy.”

The content of hallucinations ranges from brief flashes of points of light and colored lines, shapes, and patterns in simple hallucinations (phosphenes),300–302 to complex scenes of humans and animals moving,296,303–305 with a potential for bizarre, dream-like imagery of considerable detail and clarity.298 Some authors reserve the diagnosis of Charles Bonnet syndrome for patients with complex, formed hallucinations.298,299 It is clear, however, that some patients who initially have simple hallucinations eventually experience complex ones,303,305,306 as is reported in visual hallucinations in normal people subject to sensory deprivation.307 Also, because the type of release hallucination does not correlate with the site of visual loss,303 the value of such a distinction is questionable. In patients with complex hallucinations, the vision is sometimes a recognizable image from the patient's past.303,305,308

Not all patients with visual loss experience release hallucinations, so it appears that there must be other contributing factors in those patients who do have them. Increased age is one that is often cited. Indeed, in the review by Schultz and Melzack,298 80% of patients with hallucinations were more than 60 years old. However, even subjects as young as age 10 can have release hallucinations.298 Social isolation has also been mentioned as a potential factor exacerbating the sensory deprivation inherent to visual loss.309 The prospective study of Teunisse and colleagues299 found a trend toward hallucinatory patients living alone or being widowed. Although it is often stated that release hallucinations occur in persons with an otherwise sound mind, Cole309 found that cognitive impairment was a feature in 9 of his 13 patients. Schultz and Melzack,310 however, administered a psychologic test battery to 14 patients and found no evidence of cognitive dysfunction or depression. This suggests that cognitive decline is not a necessary precondition for visual release hallucinations.

Hallucinations often start in close relation to the time of visual loss. Often they follow visual loss by a period of several days or weeks, but they may be even more delayed.304,305 In cases of asymmetric binocular ophthalmic pathology, it often is not until visual loss develops in the second eye that hallucinations develop.304,306 Hallucinations and visual loss may also occur simultaneously; in such cases, it is the complaint of visual hallucinations that leads to the discovery of a visual field defect. On rare occasions, it has been documented that the hallucinations actually preceded hemianopia by a few days.300

The time during which hallucinations occur is often short, lasting a few days to a few months, although hallucinations persisting for years have also been reported.304,305 In Kölmel's series,300 the mean duration of complex hallucinations was 11.5 days. Kölmel297 also believed that the occurrence of colored patterns in hemianopic fields correlated with temporary damage to optic radiations rather than permanent destruction of striate cortex, and that these patterns were a good prognostic sign for recovery of hemianopia. The hallucinatory episode can be brief, each episode lasting a few seconds or minutes, or virtually continuous.298 Although many subjects are not troubled by their hallucinations and may even enjoy them,298 some find them annoying. Thus, although most subjects do not require treatment for hallucinations, a therapeutic trial is sometimes warranted. Unfortunately, there is no agreement on effective treatment. Good responses to anticonvulsants have been reported in some cases,305,311 but not others,304,305 and positive and negative results have also been reported with the use of haloperidol.306,309 Moving socially isolated patients into a more stimulating environment has helped on occasion.309

ANATOMY AND PATHOLOGY. Visual loss of any type can lead to release hallucinations.304 The most common site of damage in Lepore's series296 of 104 patients was cerebral, and most of these patients had had infarctions. Among ocular causes of visual loss, cataracts were most frequent. Retinal disease306 and pathology of the optic nerves and chiasm303 also have been reported. It is generally agreed that no other pathology is required for the production of release hallucinations than the disorder causing visual loss. That is, release hallucinations are not a manifestation of an underlying cerebral abnormality, but a physiologic reaction to loss of vision. It then remains to be determined, however, why not all subjects with binocular visual loss have hallucinations. Besides the influence of age and social isolation mentioned above, one report suggests that patients with hallucinations from ocular disease have a higher incidence of posterior periventricular white matter lesions seen on MRI,312 an interesting finding that needs verification.

Most series consist of patients with some degree of bilateral visual loss.303,304 The need for binocular pathology, however, has been questioned,296 and it would appear that occasionally patients who have no or minimal demonstrable visual loss have similar visual hallucinations.296,304,309 Release hallucinations can occur in normal subjects in sensory deprivation experiments,307 and it is possible that some subjects with hallucinations but without visual loss are experiencing a similar deprivation in conditions of social isolation, especially since their hallucinations can resolve with a change in surroundings.309

MECHANISMS. Schultz and Melzack298 have summarized the various theories concerning release hallucinations. In cases of intracranial pathology, hallucinations have sometimes been considered visual seizures,305 although Cogan304 suggested that release hallucinations differed from seizures by their longer duration and variability in content, as well as their association with visual loss rather than secondary motor or sensory epileptic events.298

Earlier reports of optic nerve pathology causing visual hallucinations suggested an analogous “irritation” occurring at the optic nerve.303 Others suggested that hallucinations associated with ocular pathology such as cataracts were a type of entoptic phenomenon, being the subject's interpretation of the image of retinal blood vessels, the retinal ganglion network, and other ocular structures. This theory is clearly untenable in those with enucleations. Most authors today believe that release hallucinations originate within the brain rather than in ocular structures. Visual experience is represented by patterns of coordinated impulses within the vast neural network of visual cortex. These patterns represent individual experience and normally are generated by sensory stimuli; however, it is proposed that the brain has an inherent capacity to generate such patterns spontaneously, and does so in conditions of sensory deprivation through imposed isolation or pathologic denervation.298 These patterns then correspond to hallucinations. A similar explanation has been invoked for the “phantom limb” phenomenon that sometimes follows amputation, and presumably also underlies other release phenomena such as musical hallucinations in cases of decreased hearing.313

Visual Seizures

Visual seizures are not common among the epileptic population, but they do occur and can cause confusion with migraine and release hallucinations. Indeed, much of the confusion regarding the localizing value of the content of epileptic hallucinations stems from failure to distinguish between release hallucinations and visual seizures. Weinberger and Grant's influential statement303 that “the complexity of the hallucinations has no localizing value” occurred in the context of a discussion of release hallucinations associated with optic nerve disease, not visual seizures. Clearly, release hallucinations can take on an impressive variety of manifestations, and it is the associated field defect, rather than the hallucinatory content, that holds the key to localization. The same may not be true, however, of visual hallucinations due to seizures, as Cogan304 indicated. The stimulation experiments of Penfield and Perot314 and Foerster315 suggested that simple, unformed flashes of light and colors resulted from electrical activity in primary visual cortex, whereas stimulation of visual association cortex in area 19 and temporal regions resulted in complex, formed images. A similar distinction may hold for epileptic visual hallucinations. For example, a left parietotemporal lesion was reported to cause hallucinations of objects and written words followed by post-ictal aphasia,316 and a right temporal hemorrhage caused hallucinations of a familiar face,317 whereas occipital lobe seizures were found to cause hallucinations of colored circles and spheres.318 Nevertheless, occipital lesions have also been associated with complex hallucinations.319 In such cases, spread of ictal activity into extrastriate cortex may be responsible for the formed visions.

The distinction between visual seizures and release hallucinations can be difficult when the underlying lesion is intracerebral. Cogan304 believed that release hallucinations were continuous and nonstereotyped in their content, whereas visual seizures were brief episodes with the same visual content in all seizures. Schultz and Melzack,298 however, point out that release hallucinations can sometimes be episodic rather than continuous and that their content may be repetitive. They suggest that the association with other ictal phenomena or a visual field defect is more helpful. We would add that although accompanying confusion, dysphasia, or head and eye deviation strongly support an epileptic origin, homonymous field defects indicate only the possibility of release hallucinations, since lesions that cause epilepsy may also damage the optic radiations or striate cortex. Indeed, Kölmel300 believed that complex hallucinations in hemianopia might represent a combination of ictal and release phenomena.

Although a variety of structural pathology in visual cortex may cause visual seizures, one syndrome that deserves emphasis is “benign childhood epilepsy with occipital spike-waves.”320 This idiopathic epilepsy syndrome often begins between ages 5 and 9 and ceases spontaneously in the teen-age years. Seizures are characterized by blindness or both simple and formed hallucinations, and may progress to motor or partial complex seizures. Some children develop nausea and headache after the visual seizure, creating diagnostic confusion with migraine. Electroencephalographic demonstration of occipital spike-waves during eye closure establishes the diagnosis.

Migrainous Hallucinations

Persons with migraine headaches can experience a variety of visual phenomena. These often precede the migraine headache (classic migraine) or may occur without the headache (migraine equivalent). Photopsic images are the most common, variably described as heat-wave-like shimmering, spots, or wavy lines.321 The scintillating scotoma phenomenon is a slowly enlarging blind region surrounded by a margin of sparkling lights. In some persons, the form of this hallucinatory margin can be distinctly discerned as a zig-zag pattern of lines oriented at 60° to each other,301 usually restricted to one hemifield, and on the leading edge of a C-shaped scotoma. Other terms for this pattern are “fortification spectrum” and teichopsia (Gr. teichos = town wall), derived from the resemblance of the zig-zag margin to the ground plan of town fortifications in Europe.322 Several sets of zig-zag lines may be visible and often appear to shimmer or oscillate in brightness.323 They may be black and white318 or vividly colored.322,323

The lines begin near central vision and expand toward the periphery at an accelerating rate over a period of about 20 minutes, the size of the lines also increasing with retinal eccentricity. The speed and size of these lines are predicted by the cortical magnification factor, which is a measure of the area of visual field represented in a given amount of striate cortex as a function of retinal eccentricity.301,324 This suggests that these visual hallucinations are generated by a wave of neuronal excitation spreading anteriorly through striate cortex at a constant speed, leaving in its wake a transient neuronal depression that is responsible for the temporary scotoma.323 It is also hypothesized that the zig-zag nature of the lines reflects the sensitivity to line orientation and the pattern of inhibitory interconnections within and between columns in striate cortex.323,324 Although both migraine and visual seizures can feature abnormal visions followed by headache and vomiting, Panayiotopoulos318 has suggested that the two disorders can be distinguished by the content of the hallucination, with black and white zig-zag lines dominating the former and colored circular patterns typifying the latter. This observation requires further corroboration.

Back to Top
FUNCTIONAL IMAGING AND OTHER TECHNIQUES FOR STUDYING VISION AND CORTEX
Functional imaging provides a valuable tool for assessing the activity of vision neurons in the human cerebrum in conditions of health and disease that complements the results in human brain lesion studies. PET and functional MRI studies have been used to reevaluate the topographic representation of the retina in striate cortex325,326 as well as to study the neural representations of color motion, attention, and complex visual stimuli such as faces (Fig. 16).327 For example, blood flow increases in the fusiform and lingual gyri in response to color stimuli. The use of motion stimuli activates portions of the lateral occipital gyri, at the junction of Brodmann's areas 19 and 37.217,328,329 These results correlate fairly well with the loci determined in lesion studies. Co-registering PET function with MRI-defined anatomy suggests that the human motion-selective area is most consistently related to the conjunction of the anterior limb of the inferior temporal sulcus with the lateral occipital sulcus.330 Other areas that also show changes during motion perception include V1, V2,329,330,331 and the dorsal cuneus, which may correspond to V3,330,331 among others. Viewing of optic flow increases blood flow in the dorsal cuneus, superior parietal lobe, and fusiform gyrus.332

Fig. 16. Human visual processing streams identified in PET imaging studies. Top. Lateral views of the left and right hemispheres. Bottom. Ventral views of the hemispheres. Numbers in the symbols indicate the study reporting each activated focus of increased blood flow. In some instances, multiple nearby foci of activation are shown as a single focus, representing their center of gravity. Study numbers are as follows: 1, face and location matching-to-sample; 2a, gender discrimination; 2b, face identify; 3, working memory for faces and locations; 4, shifting attention to spatial locations; 5, spatial working memory; 6, selective attention to color, shape, and velocity; 7, passive perception of color and motion; 8, passive perception of motion; 9a, word generation of object attributes from line drawings of objects; 9b, word generation of object attributes from object words; and 10, perception of word forms. (Adapted from Ungerleider LG: Functional brain imaging studies of cortical mechanisms for memory. Science 270:769, 1995)

Studies using functional MRI provide a source of data that is complementary to PET data and have shown motion-selective responses in the lateral occipitotemporal cortex, V2, and superior and inferior parietal lobules.333,334 Signal changes in lateral occipitotemporal cortex also correlate with the motion aftereffect illusion that persists after cessation of the moving display.335 Probst and co-workers336 used visual evoked potential dipole analysis to show motion responses localized to the contralateral occipitotemporoparietal border (junction of areas 19, 37, and 39). Magnetic stimulation has been employed to create temporary dysfunction within cortical areas. Stimulation over lateral occipitotemporal cortex impairs motion direction discrimination but not form discrimination in the contralateral hemifield337,338 and to a lesser degree in the ipsilateral hemifield.339

Comparative anatomic studies have provided yet another type of evidence on the organization of the human visual pathways to complement the data from functional neuroimaging and lesions studies. For example, some have inferred the location and size of a putative human area MT by comparing the histologic patterns of staining found in human brain material with the patterns observed in monkeys. Area MT is identifiable in monkeys by a pattern of dense myelination in the lower cortical laminae,226,231,232 by dense cytochrome oxidase staining,340,341 and by labeling with a monoclonal antibody (CAT-301) in Old World monkeys.342 In human lateral temporo-occipital cortex, there are similar patterns of myelin staining,341,343 cytochrome oxidase activity,341,344 and CAT-301 immunoreactivity.341

For color perception, most PET studies confirm the localization elicited from patients with cerebral achromatopsia, with increased blood flow in the lingual and fusiform gyri bilaterally,329,345–347 though some studies show changes in additional cortical regions that may vary according to the specific task used.346,347 Associating colors with line drawings of objects activated regions in the ventral temporal lobe just anterior to the region activated during color perception.348 An evoked potential study349 with implanted electrodes in epileptic patients localized responses related to color perception to the posterior fusiform gyri, but also found activity over the dorsolateral surface of the brain, which they postulated were related to selective attention to color in their task. Stimulation of electrodes near the lingual and fusiform gyri produced alterations in color perception in some of these patients.

Face perception and recognition has been the subject of numerous functional imaging experiments. Sergent and associates,350 found that identifying faces was associated with activity in the fusiform gyri and anterior temporal lobe bilaterally, whereas merely noting the gender of a face activated only ventral occipital cortex, more on the right side than on the left side. Also, object recognition activated left occipitotemporal cortex but not right, implying an anatomic dissociation between face and object perception. A ventromedial temporal focus related to face perception has been found by others as well,351–354 including a functional MRI study.355 An asymmetry in favor of right hemispheric dominance for face perception is suggested by some studies, although some left-sided activation does occur in most studies.350–352,354 Thus, functional imaging provides some circumstantial support for the claim that right hemispheric lesions alone can cause prosopagnosia.92,95,118 The hypothesized sequence of events involved in facial recognition130 has also been investigated with PET studies. One study that focused on the hippocampi concluded that the right hippocampus was involved in “primary visual processing” of faces, whereas the left was involved when explicit memory was required.354 Another study found that, although encoding of faces was associated with activity in the right medial temporal lobe, face recognition activated a large bilateral network of regions, including prefrontal, anterior cingulate, inferior parietal, ventral occipital, and even cerebellar cortex.352

In addition to these investigations of regional responses to specific perceptual stimuli, PET studies have also been used to study the basic concept of “two visual pathways.” Most of these studies have compared form and spatial vision by using tasks that require subjects to match objects either by their identity (e.g., form vision) or their location (e.g., spatial vision). These studies are in agreement, with greater activity occurring in occipitoparietal areas during spatial tasks and greater activity in occipitotemporal areas during object identification tasks.353,356,357 Other studies of activation during spatial shifts of attention confirm the role of parietal cortex in this aspect of vision.358,359

Back to Top
CONCLUSIONS
After reaching the eye, visual information converges through several parallel channels. This information converges on striate cortex, or area V1, from which it disperses to multiple functional maps located in the visual association cortex. Damage to V1 causes a contralateral scotoma or visual field defect, affecting almost all aspects of visual information. Humans have no conscious awareness of seeing in V1 scotomata, although in some cases there may be residual vision or blindsight. Lesions restricted to human visual association cortex result in clinical phenomena that can be understood in the convenient, although overly simplified, framework of two visual systems: a temporal (“what”) pathway, located in ventral and medial cortex; and a parietal (“where”) pathway located in more dorsal and lateral areas. Damage to the temporal pathway, which likely contains human homologues of simian areas V4 and IT, causes disorders of object recognition (e.g., agnosia, prosopagnosia), and color perception (e.g., cerebral achromatopsia). Damage to the parietal pathway, which probably contains human homologues of the simian area V5 complex and intraparietal areas, can impair motion perception (e.g., cerebral akinetopsia) and other aspects of visuospatial processing, including the deficits described by Balint (simultanagnosia, ocular apraxia, and optic ataxia). A variety of behavioral factors may complicate the analysis of these cases, including defective attention, denial of impairments, and covert processing. Evaluating these deficits is a challenge and demands a thorough eye examination and testing of higher visual processes and cognition with psychophysical and neuropsychologic probes. Functional neuroimaging studies with PET and MRI are adding to our understanding of both the regional anatomy of the human brain and the pathophysiology of these disorders.
Back to Top
REFERENCES

1. Schiller PH, Logothetis NK: The color-opponent and broad-based channels of the primate visual system. Trends Neurosci 11:392, 1990

2. Livingstone MS, Hubel DH: Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. J Neurosci 7:3416, 1987

3. Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC: Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1:1, 1991

4. Merigan WH, Maunsell JHR: How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annu Rev Neurosci 16:369, 1993

5. Martin KAC: Parallel pathways converge. Curr Biol 2:555, 1992

6. Lachica EA, Beck PD, Casagrande VA: Parallel pathways in macaque monkey striate cortex: anatomically defined columns in layer III. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 89:3566, 1992

7. Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M: Two cortical visual systems. In Ingle DJ, Mansfield RJW, Goodale MS (eds): The Analysis of Visual Behaviour, pp 549–586. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 1982

8. Damasio AR: Disorders of complex visual processing: Agnosias, achromatopsia, Balint's syndrome, and related difficulties of orientation and construction. In Mesulam M-M (ed): Principles of Behavioral Neurology, pp 259–288. Philadelphia, FA Davis, 1985

9. Schiller PH, Lee K: The role of the primate extrastriate area V4 in vision. Science 251:1251, 1991

10. Schiller PH: The effects of V4 and middle temporal (MT) area lesions on visual performance in the Rhesus monkey. Vis Neurosci 10:717, 1993

11. Desimone R, Schein S, Moran J, Ungerleider L: Contour, color and shape analysis beyond the striate cortex. Vision Res 25:441, 1985

12. Fujita I, Tanaka K, Ito M, Cheng K: Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Nature 360:343, 1992

13. Boussaoud D, Ungerleider LG, Desimone R: Pathways for motion analysis: Connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque. J Comp Neurol 296:462, 1990

14. Ungerleider LG, Desimone R: Cortical connections of visual area MT in the macaque. J Comp Neurol 248:190, 1986

15. Logothetis NK, Schall JD: Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. Science 256:761, 1989

16. Brodmann K: Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Printzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Leipzig, JA Barth, 1909

17. Inouye T: Die Sehstorungen bei Schussverletzungen der kortikalen Sesphare. Leipzig, Engelmann, 1909

18. Holmes G, Lister WT: Disturbances of vision from cerebral lesions with special reference to the cortical representation of the macula. Brain 39:34, 1916

19. Horton JC, Hoyt WF: The representation of the visual field in human striate cortex: A revision of the classic Holmes map. Arch Ophthalmol 109:816, 1991

20. Anderson DR: Testing the Field of Vision. St. Louis, CV Mosby, 1982

21. Smith CG, Richardson WFG: The course and distribution of the arteries supplying the visual (striate) cortex. Am J Ophthalmol 61:1391, 1966

22. Huber A: Homonymous hemianopia after occipital lobectomy. Am J Ophthalmol 54:623, 1962

23. Tootell RBH, Switkes E, Silverman MS, Hamilton SL: Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex: II. Retinotopic organization. J Neurosci 8:1531, 1988

24. Spalding JMK: Wounds of the visual pathway: Part II. The striate cortex. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 15:169, 1952

25. Benton S, Levy I, Swash M: Vision in the temporal crescent in occipital infarction. Brain 103:83, 1980

26. Halpern JI, Sedler RR: Traumatic bilateral homonymous hemianopic scotomas. Ann Ophthalmol 12:1022, 1980

27. Sandifer PH: Anosognosia and disorders of body scheme. Brain 69:122, 1946

28. Geschwind N: Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain 88:17, 1965

29. McDaniel KD, McDaniel LD: Anton's syndrome in a patient with posttraumatic optic neuropathy and bifrontal contusions. Arch Neurol 48:101, 1991

30. Hartmann JA, Wolz WA, Roeltgen DP, Loverso FL: Denial of visual perception. Brain Cogn 16:29, 1991

31. Teuber HL: Alteration of perception and memory in man. In Weiskrantz L (ed): Analysis of Behavioural Change. New York, Harper & Row, 1968

32. Pöppel E, Held R, Frost D: Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man. Nature 243:295, 1973

33. Weiskrantz L, Warrington EK, Sanders MD, Marshall J: Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain 97:709, 1974

34. Weiskrantz L: Residual vision in a scotoma: A follow-up study of “form” discrimination. Brain 110:77, 1987

35. Sanders MD, Warrington E, Marshall J, Weiskrantz L: “Blindsight”: Vision in a field defect. Lancet April 20:707, 1974

36. Blythe IM, Kennard C, Ruddock KH: Residual vision in patients with retrogeniculate lesions of the visual pathways. Brain 110:887, 1987

37. Barbur JL, Ruddock KH, Waterfield VA: Human visual responses in the absence of the geniculo-calcarine projection. Brain 103:905, 1980

38. Barbur JL, Forsyth PM, Findlay JM: Human saccadic eye movements in the absence of the geniculocalcarine projection. Brain 111:63, 1988

39. Weiskrantz L: Outlooks for blindsight: Explicit methodologies for implicit processes. Proc R Soc Lond [B] 239:257, 1990

40. Perenin M-T: Visual functions within the hemianopic field following early cerebral hemidecortication in man: II. Pattern discrimination. Neuropsychologia 16:697, 1978

41. Stoerig P, Hébner M, Pöppel E: Signal detection analysis of residual vision in a field defect due to a post-geniculate lesion. Neuropsychologia 23:589, 1985

42. Stoerig P, Cowey A: Increment-threshold spectral sensitivity in blindsight: Evidence for colour opponency. Brain 114:1487, 1991

43. Campion J, Latto R, Smith YM: Is blindsight an effect of scattered light, spared cortex, and near-threshold vision? Behav Brain Sci 6:423, 1983

44. Meeres SL, Graves RE: Localization of unseen stimuli by humans with normal vision. Neuropsychologia 28:1231, 1990

45. Stoerig P: Chromaticity and achromaticity: Evidence for a functional differentiation in visual field defects. Brain 110:869, 1987

46. Balliet R, Blood KMT, Bach-Y-Rita P: Visual field rehabilitation in the cortically blind? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 48:1113, 1985

47. Perenin M-T, Ruel J, Hècaen H: Residual visual capacities in a case of cortical blindness. Cortex 6:605, 1980

48. Perenin M-T, Jeannerod M: Residual function in cortically blind hemifields. Neuropsychologia 13:1, 1975

49. Perenin M-T, Jeannerod M: Visual functions within the hemianopic field following early cerebral hemidecortication in man: I. Spatial localization. Neuropsychologia 16:1, 1978

50. Meienberg O, Zangemeister WH, Rosenberg M et al: Saccadic eye movement strategies in patients with homonymous hemianopia. Ann Neurol 9:537, 1981

51. Corbetta M, Marzi CA, Tassinari G, Aglioti S: Effectiveness of different task paradigms in revealing blindsight. Brain 113:603, 1990

52. Ptito A, Lepore F, Ptito M, Lassonde M: Target detection and movement discrimination in the blind field of hemispherectomized patients. Brain 114:497, 1991

53. Bridgeman B, Staggs D: Plasticity in human blindsight. Vision Res 22:1199, 1982

54. Riddoch G: Dissociation of visual perception due to occipital injuries with especial reference to appreciation of movement. Brain 40:15, 1917

55. Perenin M-T: Discrimination of motion direction in perimetrically blind fields. Neuroreport 2:397, 1991

56. Heide W, Koenig E, Dichgans J: Optokinetic nystagmus, self-motion sensation and their after effects in patients with occipito-parietal lesions. Clin Vision Sci 5:145, 1990

57. ter Braak JWG, Schenk VWD, van Vliet AGM: Visual reactions in a case of long-lasting cortical blindness. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 34:140, 1971

58. Pizzamiglio L, Antonucci G, Francia A: Response of the cortically blind hemifields to a moving stimulus. Cortex 20:89, 1984

59. Mestre DR, Brouchon M, Ceccaldi M, Poncet M: Perception of optical flow in cortical blindness: A case report. Neuropsychologia 30:783, 1992

60. Torjussen T: Visual processing in cortically blind hemifields. Neuropsychologia 16:15, 1978

61. Ptito A, Lassonde M, Lepore F, Ptito M: Visual discrimination in hemispherectomized patients. Neuropsychologia 25:869, 1987

62. Hess RF, Pointer JS: Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity in hemianopia. Brain 112:871, 1989

63. Rafal R, Smith J, Krantz J et al: Extrageniculate vision in hemianopic humans: Saccade inhibition by signals in the blind field. Science 250:118, 1990

64. Marzi CA, Tassinari G, Agliotti S, Lutzemberger L: Spatial summation across the vertical meridian in hemianopics: A test of blindsight. Neuropsychologia 24:749, 1986

65. Zihl J: “Blindsight”: Improvement of visually guided eye movements by systematic practice in patients with cerebral blindness. Neuropsychologia 18:71, 1980

66. Zihl J, Werth R: Contributions to the study of “blindsight”: I. Can stray light account for saccadic localization in patients with postgeniculate visual field defects? Neuropsychologia 22:1, 1984

67. Zihl J, Werth R: Contributions to the study of “blindsight”: II. The role of specific practice for saccadic localization in patients with postgeniculate visual field defects. Neuropsychologia 22:13, 1984

68. Zihl J, von Cramon D: Restitution of visual function in patients with cerebral blindness. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 42:312, 1979

69. Zihl J: Recovery of visual function in patients with cerebral blindness. Exp Brain Res 24:159, 1981

70. Bender DB: Electrophysiological and behavioural experiments on the primate pulvinar. Prog Brain Res 75:55, 1988

71. Bender DB: Visual activation of neurons in the primate pulvinar depends on cortex but not colliculus. Brain Res 279:258, 1983

72. Blythe IM, Bromley JM, Kennard C, Ruddock KH: Visual discrimination of target displacement remains after damage to the striate cortex in humans. Nature 320:619, 1986

73. Cowey A, Stoerig P: The neurobiology of blindsight. Trends Neurosci 14:140, 1991

74. Weiskrantz L: Behavioural analysis of the monkey's visual nervous system. Proc Royal Soc London B Biol Sci 182:427, 1972

75. Mohler CW, Wurtz RH: Role of striate cortex and superior colliculus in visual guidance of saccadic eye movements in monkey. J Neurophysiol 40:74, 1977

76. Keating EG: Residual spatial vision in the monkey after removal of striate and preoccipital cortex. Brain Res 187:271, 1980

77. Zee DS, Tusa RJ, Herdman SJ et al: Effects of occipital lobectomy upon eye movements in primate. J Neurophysiol 58:883, 1987

78. Segraves MA, Goldberg ME, Deng S-Y et al: The role of striate cortex in the guidance of eye movements in the monkey. J Neurosci 7:3040, 1987

79. Gross CG: Contributions of striate cortex and the superior colliculus to visual functions in area MT, the superior temporal polysensory area and inferior temporal cortex. Neuropsychologia 29:497, 1991

80. Rodman HR Gross CG, Albright TD: Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque: I. Effects of striate cortex removal. J Neurosci 9:2033, 1989

81. Girard P, Salin PA, Bullier J: Visual activity in areas V3a and V3 during reversible inactivation of area V1 in the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 66:1493, 1991

82. Girard P, Salin PA, Bullier J: Response selectivity of neurons in area MT of the macaque monkey during reversible inactivation of area V1. J Neurophysiol 67:1437, 1992

83. Rodman HR, Gross CG, Albright TD: Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque: II. Effects of superior colliculus removal. J Neurosci 10:1154, 1990

84. Fendrich R, Wessinger CM, Gazzaniga MS: Residual vision in a scotoma: Implications for blindsight. Science 258:1489, 1992

85. Celesia GG, Bushnell D, Toleikis SC, Brigell MG: Cortical blindness and residual vision: Is the “second” visual system in humans capable of more than rudimentary visual perception? Neurology 41:862, 1991

85a. Merigan WH, Nealey TA, Maunsell JHR: Visual effects of lesions of cortical area V2 in macaques. J Neurosci 13:3180, 1993

86. Braddick O, Atkinson J, Hood B et al: Possible blindsight in infants lacking one cerebral hemisphere. Nature 360:461, 1992

87. Lissauer H: Ein fall von Seelenblindheit nebst einem Bitrag zur Theorie derselben. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 2:22, 1890

87a. Bodamer J: Prosopagnosie. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 179:6, 1947

88. Benton A: Facial recognition 1990. Cortex 26:491, 1990

89. Damasio AR: Prosopagnosia. Trends Neurosci 8:132, 1985

90. Young AW, Ellis HD: Childhood prosopagnosia. Brain Cogn 9:16, 1989

91. de Haan EHF, Campbell R: A fifteen year follow-up of a case of developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex 27:489, 1991

92. Landis T, Cummings JL, Christen L et al: Are unilateral right posterior lesions sufficient to cause prosopagnosia?: Clinical and radiological findings in six additional patients. Cortex 22:243, 1986

93. Bruyer R, Laterre C, Seron X et al: A case of prosopagnosia with some preserved covert remembrance of familiar faces. Brain Cogn 2:257, 1983

94. de Haan EHF, Young AW, Newcombe F: Face recognition without awareness. Cogn Neuropsychol 4:385, 1987

95. Sergent J, Villemure J-G: Prosopagnosia in a right hemispherectomized patient. Brain 112:975, 1989

96. Sergent J, Poncet M: From covert to overt recognition of faces in a prosopagnosic patient. Brain 113:989, 1990

97. Campbell R, Landis T, Regard M: Face recognition and lipreading: A neurological dissociation. Brain 109:509, 1986

98. Lhermitte F, Chain F, Escourolle R et al: Ötude anatomo-clinique d'un cas de prosopagnosie. Rev Neurol 126:329, 1972

99. Whitely AM, Warrington EK: Prosopagnosia: A clinical, psychological and anatomical study of three patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 40:395, 1977

100. Damasio AR, Damasio H, van Hoesen GW: Prosopagnosia: Anatomic basis and behavioural mechanisms. Neurology 32:331, 1982

101. Farah MJ: Visual agnosia: Disorders of visual recognition and what they tell us about normal vision. Cambridge, MIT Press, 1990

102. de Renzi E: Current issues in prosopagnosia. In Ellis HD, Jeeves MA, Newcome F, Young A (eds): Aspects of Face Processing. Martinus Nijhoff, Dordecht, 1986

103. McNeil JE, Warrington EK: Prosopagnosia: A face-specific disorder. Q J Exp Psychol 46A:1, 1993

104. Malone DR, Morris HH, Kay MC, Levin HS: Prosopagnosia: A double dissociation between the recognition of familiar and unfamiliar faces. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 45:820, 1982

105. Levine DN, Warach J, Farah M: Two visual systems in mental imagery: Dissociation of “what” and “where” in imagery disorders due to bilateral posterior cerebral lesions. Neurology 35:1010, 1985

106. Rizzo M, Hurtig R, Damasio AR: The role of scanpaths in facial recognition and learning. Ann Neurol 22:41, 1987

107. Bauer RM, Verfaellie M: Electrodermal discrimination of familiar but not unfamiliar faces in prosopagnosia. Brain Cogn 8:240, 1988

108. Renault B, Signoret J-L, DeBruille B et al: Brain potentials reveal covert facial recognition in prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia 27:905, 1989

109. Rizzo M, Corbett JJ, Thompson HS, Damasio AR: Spatial contrast sensitivity in facial recognition. Neurology 36:1254, 1986

110. Bauer RM: Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: A neuropsychological application of the guilty knowledge test. Neuropsychologia 22:457, 1984

111. Damasio H, Frank R: Three-dimensional in vivo mapping of brain lesions in humans. Arch Neurol 49:137, 1992

112. Meadows JC: The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 37:489, 1974

113. Benson DF, Segarra J, Albert ML: Visual agnosia-prosopagnosia. Arch Neurol 30:307, 1974

114. Tyrell PJ, Warrington EK, Frackowiak RSJ, Rossor MN: Progressive degeneration of the right temporal lobe studied with positron emission tomography. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 53:1048, 1990

115. Kay MC, Levin HS: Prosopagnosia. Am J Ophthalmol 94:75, 1982

116. Hècaen H, Angelergues R: Agnosia for faces (prosopagnosia). Arch Neurol 7:92, 1962

117. Assal G: Regression des troubles de la reconnaissance des physiognomies et de la mèmoire topographique chez un malade opèrè d'un hèmatome intracèrèbral parièto-temporal droit. Rev Neurol 121:184, 1969

118. de Renzi E: Prosopagnosia in two patients with CT scan evidence of damage confined to the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 24:385, 1986

119. Michel F, Perenin M-T, Sieroff E: Prosopagnosie sans hèmianopsie après lèsion unilatèrale occipito-temporale droite. Rev Neurol 142:545, 1986

120. Michel F, Poncet M, Signoret JL: Les lèsions responsables de la prosopagnosie sont-elles toujours bilaterales? Rev Neurol 146:764, 1989

121. Landis T, Regard M, Bliestle A, Kleihues P: Prosopagnosia and agnosia for non-canonical views. Brain 111:1287, 1988

122. de Renzi E, Faglioni P, Grossi D, Nichelli P: Apperceptive and associative forms of prosopagnosia. Cortex 27:213, 1991

123. Tranel D, Damasio AR: Knowledge without awareness: An autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics. Science 228:1453, 1985

124. Ettlin TM, Beckson M, Benson DF et al: Prosopagnosia: A bihemispheric disorder. Cortex 28:129, 1992

125. Bruyer R: Covert facial recognition in prosopagnosia: A review. Brain Cogn 15:223, 1991

126. McNeil JE, Warrington EK: Prosopagnosia: A reclassification. Q J Exp Psychol 43A:267, 1991

127. Bay E: Disturbances of visual perception and their examination. Brain 76:515, 1953

128. Cohn R, Neumann MA, Wood DH: Prosopagnosia: A clinicopathological study. Ann Neurol 1:177, 1977

129. Hoff H, Potzl O: Über eine optisch-agnostische Störung des ‘Physiognomie-Gedachtnibes’ (Bezeihungen zur Réckbildung ein Wortblindheit. Z Gesamte Neurol Psychiat 159:367, 1937

130. Bruce V, Young A: Understanding face recognition. Br J Psychol 77:305, 1986

131. Levine DN, Calvanio R: Prosopagnosia: A defect in visual configural processing. Brain Cogn 10:149, 1989

132. Rentschler I, Treutwein B, Landis T: Dissociation of local and global processing in visual agnosia. Vision Res 34:963, 1994

133. Pallis CA: Impaired identification of faces and places with agnosia for colours. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 18:218, 1955

134. Korner F, Regli F, Haynal A: Eine durch Farbinnstörung, Prosopagnosie und Orienterungs-störung charakterisierte visuelle Agnosie. Arch Psychiatr NervKrankh 209:1, 1967

135. Meadows JC: Disturbed perception of colours associated with localized cerebral lesions. Brain 97:615, 1974

136. MacKay G, Dunlop JC: The cerebral lesions in a case of complete acquired colour-blindness. Scott Med Surg J 5:503, 1899

137. Rizzo M, Smith V, Pokorny J, Damasio AR: Color perception profiles in central achromatopsia. Neurology 43:995, 1993

138. Lenz G: Zwei Sectionsfalle doppelseitiger zentrale Farbenhemianopsie. Z Gesamte Neurol Psychiat 71:135, 1921

139. Sacks O: The case of the colour-blind painter. In: An Anthropologist on Mars. New York, Alfred A Knopf, 1995

140. Critchley M: Acquired anomalies of colour perception of central origin. Brain 88:711, 1965

141. Victor JD, Maiese K, Shapley R et al: Acquired central dyschromatopsia: Analysis of a case with preservation of color discrimination. Clin Vision Sci 4:183, 1989

142. Heywood CA, Cowey A, Newcombe F: Chromatic discrimination in a cortically blind observer. Eur J Neurosci 3:802, 1991

143. Ogden JA: Visual object agnosia, prosopagnosia, achromatopsia, loss of visual imagery, and autobiographical amnesia following recovery from cortical blindness: Case M.H. Neuropsychologia 31:571, 1993

144. Green GJ, Lessell S: Acquired cerebral dyschromatopsia. Arch Ophthalmol 95:121, 1977

145. Paulson HL, Galetta SL, Grossman M, Alavi A: Hemiachromatopsia of unilateral occipitotemporal infarcts. Am J Ophthalmol 118:518, 1994

146. Albert ML, Reches A, Silverberg R: Hemianopic colour blindness. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 38:546, 1975

147. Kölmel HW: Pure homonymous hemiachromatopsia: Findings with neuro-ophthalmologic examination and imaging procedures. Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci 237:237, 1988

148. Damasio A, Yamada T, Damasio H et al: Central achromatopsia: Behavioral, anatomic, and physiologic aspects. Neurology 30:1064, 1980

149. Zeki SM: A century of cerebral achromatopsia. Brain 113:1721, 1990

150. Pearlman AL, Birch J, Meadows JC: Cerebral color blindness: An acquired defect in hue discrimination. Ann Neurol 5:253, 1979

151. Lueck CJ, Zeki S, Friston KJ et al: The colour center in the cerebral cortex of man. Nature 340:386, 1989

152. de Renzi E, Spinnler H: Impaired performance in colour tasks in patients with hemispheric damage. Cortex 3:194, 1967

153. Aldrich MS, Vanderzant CW, Alessi AG et al: Ictal cortical blindness with permanent visual loss. Epilepsia 30:116, 1989

154. Freedman L, Costa L: Pure alexia and right hemiachromatopsia in posterior dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55:500, 1992

155. Lawden MC, Cleland PG: Achromatopsia in the aura of migraine. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 56:708, 1993

156. Heywood CA, Wilson B, Cowey A: A case study of cortical colour “blindness” with relatively intact achromatic discrimination. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 50:22, 1987

157. Wyszecki G, Stiles WS: Color science: Concepts and methods, quantitative data and formulae. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1982

158. Kaiser PK, Boynton RM: Human color vision, 2nd ed. Optical Society of America, USA, 1996

159. Land EH: Recent advances in retinex theory. Vision Res 26:7, 1986

160. Land EH, Hubel DH, Livingstone MS et al: Colourgenerating interactions across the corpus callosum. Nature 303:616, 1983

161. Zeki SM: Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: The reaction of cells in monkey visual cortex to wavelengths and colours. Neuroscience 9:741, 1983

162. Zeki SM: Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: The responses of wavelength-selective and colour-coded cells in monkey visual cortex to changes in wavelength composition. Neuroscience 9:767, 1983

163. Schein SJ, Desimone R: Spectral properties of V4 neurons in the macaque. J Neurosci 10:3369, 1990

164. Heywood CA, Cowey A: On the role of cortical area V4 in the discrimination of hue and pattern in macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 7:2601, 1987

165. Heywood CA, Gadotti A, Cowey A: Cortical area V4 and its role in the perception of color. J Neurosci 12:4056, 1992

166. Wild HM, Butler SR, Carden D, Kulikowski JJ: Primate cortical area V4 important for colour constancy but not wavelength discrimination. Nature 313:133, 1985

167. Dean P: Visual cortex ablation and thresholds for successively presented stimuli in Rhesus monkeys: II. Hue. Exp Brain Res 35:69, 1979

168. Holmes G: Pure word blindness. Folia Psychiatr Neurol Neurochir Neerl 53:279, 1950

169. Geschwind N, Fusillo M: Color-naming defects in association with alexia. Arch Neurol 15:137, 1966

170. Oxbury JM, Oxbury SM, Humphrey NK: Varieties of colour anomia. Brain 92:847, 1969

171. de Vreese LP: Two systems for color-naming defects: Verbal disconnection versus colour imagery disorder. Neuropsychologia 29:1, 1991

172. Davidoff JB, Ostergaard AL: Colour anomia resulting from weakened short-term colour memory. Brain 107:415, 1984

173. Kinsbourne M, Warrington EK: Observations on colour agnosia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 27:296, 1964

174. Black SE, Behrmann M: Localization in alexia. In: Localization and Neuroimaging in Neuropsychology, pp 331–376. New York, Academic Press, 1994

175. Galaburda AM, Rosen GD, Drislane FW, Livingstone MS: Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia (abstr). Soc Neurosci 17:20, 1991

176. Livingstone MS, Rosen GD, Drislane FW, Galaburda AM: Physiological and anatomical evidence for magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 88:7943, 1991

177. Dejerine J: Sur un cas de cecite verbale avec agraphie, suive d'autopsie. CR Societè du Biologie 43:197, 1891

178. Dejerine J: Contributions a l'ètude anatomopathologique et clinique des differentes varietes de cecite verbale. Memoires de la Societè Biologique 44:61, 1892

179. Damasio AR, Damasio H: The anatomic basis of pure alexia. Neurology 33:1573, 1983

180. Binder JR, Mohr JP: The topography of callosal reading pathways: A case control analysis. Brain 115:1807, 1992

181. Bub DN, Black SE, Howell J: Word recognition and orthographic context effects in a letter-by-letter reader. Brain Lang 36:357, 1989

182. Coslett HB, Saffran EM, Greenbaum S, Schwartz H: Reading in pure alexia. Brain 116:21, 1993

183. Greenblatt SH: Alexia without agraphia or hemianopia. Brain 96:307, 1973

184. Vincent FM, Sadowsky CH, Saunders RL, Reeves AG: Alexia without agraphia, hemianopia, or color-naming defect: A disconnection syndrome. Neurology 27:689, 1977

185. Henderson VW, Friedman RB, Teng EL, Weiner JM: Left hemisphere pathways in reading: Inferences from pure alexia without hemianopia. Neurology 35:962, 1985

186. Iragui V, Kritchevsky M: Alexia without agraphia or hemianopia in parietal infarction. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 54:841, 1991

187. de Renzi E, Zambolin A, Crisi G: The pattern of neuropsychological impairment associated with left posterior cerebral artery infarcts. Brain 110:1099, 1987

188. Albert ML, Yamadori A, Gardner H, Howes D: Comprehension in alexia. Brain 96:317, 1973

189. Coslett HB, Saffran EM: Evidence for preserved reading in pure alexia. Brain 112:327, 1989

190. Caplan LR, Hedley-White T: Cuing and memory dysfunction in alexia without agraphia: A case report. Brain 97:251, 1974

191. Warrington EK, Shallice T: Word-form dyslexia. Brain 103:99, 1980

192. Behrmann M, Black SE, Bub DN: The evolution of pure alexia: A longitudinal study of recovery. Brain Lang 39:405, 1990

193. Silver FL, Chawluk JB, Bosley TM et al: Resolving metabolic abnormalities in a case of pure alexia. Neurology 38:731, 1988

194. Stommel EW, Friedman RJ, Reeves AG: Alexia without agraphia associated with spleniogeniculate infarction. Neurology 41:587, 1991

195. Vaina LM: Functional segregation of color and motion processing in the human visual cortex: Clinical evidence. Cereb Cortex 5:555, 1994

196. Greenblatt SH: Localization of lesions in alexia. In Kertesz A (ed): Localization in Neuropsychology, p 323–356. New York, Academic Press, 1983

197. Gazzaniga MS, Freedman H: Observations on visual processes after posterior callosal section. Neurology 23:1126, 1973

198. Castro-Caldas A, Salgado V: Right hemifield alexia without hemianopia. Arch Neurol 41:84, 1984

199. Binder JR, Lazar RM, Tatemichi TK et al: Left hemiparalexia. Neurology 42:562, 1992

200. Benson DF: Alexia. In Bruyn GW, Klawans HL, Vinken PJ (eds): Handbook of Clinical Neurology, pp 433–455. New York, Elsevier, 1985

201. Kawahata N, Nagata K: Alexia with agraphia due to the left posterior inferior temporal lobe lesion: Neuropsychological analysis and its pathogenetic mechanisms. Brain Lang 33:296, 1988

202. Benson DF, Brown J, Tomlinson EB: Varieties of alexia: Word and letter blindness. Neurology 21:951, 1971

203. Benson DF: The third alexia. Arch Neurol 34:327, 1977

204. Kirkham TH: The ocular symptomology of pituitary tumors. Proc R Soc Med 65:517, 1972

205. Shallice T, Warrington EK: The possible role of selective attention in acquired dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 15:31, 1977

206. Levine DN, Calvanio R: A study of the visual defect in verbal alexia-simultanagnosia. Brain 101:65, 1978

207. Behrmann M, Moscovitch M, Black SE, Mozer M: Perceptual and conceptual factors in neglect dyslexia: Two contrasting case studies. Brain 113:1163, 1990

208. Bálint R: Seelenlahmung des ‘Schauens’, optische Ataxie, räumliche Storung der Aufmerksamkeit. Monatschrift für Psychiatrie und Neurologie 25:51, 1909

209. Wolpert T: Die Simultanagosie. Zeischrift für gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 93:397, 1924

210. Holmes G: Disturbance of vision by cerebral lesions. Br J Ophthalmol 2:353, 1918

210a. Hécaen H, de Ajuriaguerra J: Bálint's syndrome (psychic paralysis of visual fixation) and its minor forms. Brain 77:373, 1954

211. Luria AR: Disorders of ‘simultaneous perception’ in a case of bilateral occipitoparietal brain injury. Brain 82:437, 1959

212. Luria AR, Pravdina-Vinarskaya EN, Yarbus AL: Disturbances of ocular movement in a case of simultanagnosia. Brain 86:219, 1962

213. Rizzo M: Bálint's syndrome and associated visuospatial disorders. In Kennard C (ed): Bailliere's International Practice and Research, pp 415–437. Philadelphia, WB Saunders, 1993

214. Ball KK, Roenker DL, Bruni JR: Developmental changes in attention and visual search throughout adulthood. In JT Enns (ed): The Development of Attention: Research and Theory, pp 489–507. New York, Elsevier NorthHolland, 1990

215. Moran J, Desimone R: Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science 229:782, 1985

216. Spitzer J, Desimone R, Moran J: Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance. Science 240:338, 1988

217. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Dobmeyer S et al: Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color and speed: Functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. J Neurosci 11:2383, 1991

218. Damasio AR, Benton AL: Impairment of hand movements under visual guidance. Neurology 29:170, 1979

219. Zihl J, von Cramon D, Mai N: Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. Brain 106:313, 1983

220. Montero J, Pena J, Genis D et al: Bálint's syndrome: Report of four cases with watershed parieto-occipital lesions from vertebrobasilar ischemia or systemic hypotension. Acta Neurol Belg 82:270, 1982

221. Hijdra A, Meerwaldt JD: Bálint's syndrome in a man with border-zone infarcts caused by atrial fibrillation. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 86:51, 1984

222. Schnider A, Landis T, Regard M: Bálint's syndrome in subacute HIV encephalitis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 54:822, 1991

223. Hof PR, Bouras C, Constantinidis J et al: Selective disconnection of specific visual association pathways in cases of Alzheimer's disease presenting the Bálint's syndrome. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 49:168, 1990

224. Mendez MF, Mendez MA, Martin R et al: Complex visual disturbances in Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 40:439, 1990

225. Tranel D: Assessment of higher-order visual function. Curr Opin Ophthalmol 5:29, 1994

226. Dubner R, Zeki SM: Response properties and receptive fields of cells in an anatomically defined region of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey. Brain Res 35:528, 1971

227. Maunsell JHR, van Essen DC: Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey: I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. J Neurophysiol 49:1127, 1983

228. Felleman DJ, Kaas JH: Receptive field properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area (MT) of owl monkeys. J Neurophysiol 52:488, 1984

229. Albright TD: Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. Science 255:1141, 1992

230. Lagae L, Raiguel S, Orban GA: Speed and direction selectivity of macaque middle temporal neurons. J Neurophysiol 69:19, 1993

231. van Essen DC, Maunsell JHR, Bixby JL: The middle temporal visual area in the macaque: Myeloarchitecture, connections, functional properties and topographic organization. J Comp Neurol 199:293, 1981

232. Maunsell JHR, van Essen DC: The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 3:2563, 1983

233. Tanaka K, Sugita Y, Moriya M, Saito H-A: Analysis of object motion in the ventral part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 69:128, 1993

234. Tanaka K, Saito H-A: Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 62:626, 1989

235. Duffy CJ, Wurtz RH: Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli: I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. J Neurophysiol 65:1329, 1991

236. Duffy CJ, Wurtz RH: Response of monkey MST neurons to optic flow stimuli with shifted centers of motion. J Neurosci 15:5192, 1995

237. Graziano MSA, Andersen RA, Snowden RJ: Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions. J Neurosci 14:54, 1994

238. Colby CL, Duhamel J-R, Goldberg ME: Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: Anatomic location and visual response properties. J Neurophysiol 69:902, 1993

239. Bruce CJ, Desimone R, Gross CG: Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. J Neurophysiol 46:369, 1981

240. Oram MW, Perrett DI, Hietanen JK: Directional tuning of motion-sensitive cells in the anterior superior temporal polysensory area of the macaque. Exp Brain Res 97:274, 1993

241. Perrett DI, Smith PAJ, Mistlin AJ et al: Visual analysis of body movements by neurons in the temporal cortex of the macaque monkey: A preliminary report. Behav Brain Res 16:153, 1985

242. Sáry G, Vogels R, Orban G: Cue-invariant shape selectivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons. Science 260:995, 1993

243. Sáry G, Vogels R, Kovcs G, Orban GA: Responses of monkey inferior temporal neurons to luminance-, motion-, and texture-defined gratings. J Neurophysiol 73:1341, 1995

244. Snowden RJ, Treue S, Erickson RG, Andersen RA: The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion. J Neurosci 11:2766, 1991

245. Qian N, Andersen RA: Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals: II. Physiology. J Neurosci 14:7367, 1994

246. Qian N, Andersen RA, Adelson EH: Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals: I. Psychophysics. J Neurosci 14:7357, 1994

247. Movshon JA, Adelson EH, Gizzi MS, Newsome WT: The analysis of moving visual patterns. In Chagas C, Gattass R, Gross CG (eds): Study Group on Pattern Recognition Mechanisms. Vatican City, Pontifica Academia Scientiarum, 1985

248. Rodman HR, Albright TD: Single-unit analysis of pattern-motion selective properties in the middle temporal visual area (MT). Exp Brain Res 75:53, 1989

249. Bradley DC, Qian N, Andersen RA: Integration of motion and stereopsis in middle temporal cortical area of macaques. Nature 373:609, 1995

250. Olavarria JF, DeYoe EA, Knierim JJ et al: Neural responses to visual texture patterns in middle temporal area of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 68:164, 1992

251. Born RT, Tootell RBH: Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area. Nature 357:497, 1992

252. Allman J, Miezin F, McGuinness E: Direction- and velocity-specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field in the middle temporal area (MT). Perception 14:105, 1985

253. Tanaka K, Hikosaka K, Saito H-A et al: Analysis of local and wide-field movements in the superior temporal visual areas of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 6:134, 1986

254. Newsome WT, Wurtz RH, Dürsteler MR, Mikami A: Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 5:825, 1985

255. Dürsteler MR, Wurtz RH, Newsome WT: Directional pursuit deficits following lesions of the foveal representation within the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 57:1262, 1987

256. Dürsteler MR, Wurtz RH: Pursuit and optokinetic deficits following chemical lesions of cortical areas MT and MST. J Neurophysiol 60:940, 1988

257. Schiller PH, Lee K: The effects of lateral geniculate nucleus, area V4, and middle temporal (MT) lesions on visually guided eye movements. Vis Neurosci 11:229, 1994

258. Newsome WT, Parè EB: A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J Neurosci 8:2201, 1988

259. Yamasaki DS, Wurtz RH: Recovery of function after lesions in the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey. J Neurophysiol 66:651, 1991

260. Cowey A, Marcar VL: The effect of removing superior temporal cortical areas in the macaque monkey: I. Motion discrimination using simple dots. Eur J Neurosci 4:1219, 1992

261. Marcar VL, Cowey A: The effect of removing superior temporal cortical areas in the macaque monkey: II. Motion discrimination using random textured displays. Eur J Neurosci 4:1228, 1992

262. Britten KH, Newsome WT, Saunders RC: Effects of inferotemporal cortex lesions on form-from-motion discrimination in monkeys. Exp Brain Res 88:292, 1992

263. Salzman CD, Britten KH, Newsome WT: Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction. Nature 346:174, 1990

264. Salzman CD, Murasugi CM, Britten KH, Newsome WT: Microstimulation in visual area MT: Effects on direction discrimination performance. J Neurosci 12:2331, 1992

265. Murasugi CM, Salzman CD, Newsome WT: Microstimulation in visual area MT: Effects of varying pulse amplitude and frequency. J Neurosci 13:1719, 1993

266. Newsome WT, Britten KH, Movshon JA: Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature 341:52, 1989

267. Celebrini S, Newsome WT: Neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity to motion signals in extrastriate area MST of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 14:4109, 1994

268. Nakayama K: Biological image motion processing: A review. Vision Res 25:625, 1985

269. Zeki S: Cerebral akinetopsia (visual motion blindness): A review. Brain 114:811, 1991

270. Rizzo M, Nawrot M, Zihl J: Motion and shape perception in cerebral akinetopsia. Brain 118:1105, 1995

271. Zihl J, von Cramon D, Mai N, Schmid C: Disturbance of movement vision after bilateral posterior brain damage: Further evidence and follow-up observations. Brain 114:2235, 1991

272. Hess RH, Baker CL Jr, Zihl J: The “motion-blind” patient: Low-level spatial and temporal filters. J Neurosci 9:1628, 1989

273. Baker CL Jr, Hess RF, Zihl J: Residual motion perception in a “motion-blind” patient, assessed with limited-lifetime random dot stimuli. J Neurosci 11:454, 1991

274. Plant GT, Laxer KD, Barbaro NM et al: Impaired visual motion perception in the contralateral hemifield following unilateral posterior cerebral lesions in humans. Brain 116:1303, 1993

275. Greenlee MW, Lang HJ, Mergner T, Seeger W: Visual short-term memory of stimulus velocity in patients with unilateral posterior brain damage. J Neurosci 15:2287, 1995

276. Barton JJS, Sharpe JA, Raymond JE: Retinotopic and directional defects in motion discrimination in humans with cerebral lesions. Ann Neurol 37:665, 1995

277. Vaina LM: Selective impairment of visual motion interpretation following lesions of the right occipito-parietal area in humans. Biol Cybern 61:347, 1989

278. Regan D, Giaschi D, Sharpe JA, Hong XH: Visual processing of motion-defined form: Selective failure in patients with parietotemporal lesions. J Neurosci 12:2198, 1992

279. Nawrot M, Rizzo M, Damasio H: Motion perception in humans with focal cerebral lesions (abstr). Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 34 (suppl): 1231, 1993

280. Critchley M: Types of visual perseveration: “Paliopsia” and “illusory visual spread.” Brain 74:267, 1951

281. Bender MB, Feldman M, Sobin AJ: Palinopsia. Brain 91:321, 1968

282. Bender MB: Polyopia and monocular diplopia of cerebral origin. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 54:323, 1945

283. Michel EM, Troost BT: Palinopsia: Cerebral localization with computed tomography. Neurology 30:887, 1980

284. Kinsbourne M, Warrington EK: A study of visual perseveration. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 26:468, 1963

285. Cummings JL, Syndulko K, Goldberg Z, Treiman DM: Palinopsia reconsidered. Neurology 32:444, 1982

286. Meadows JC, Munro SSF: Palinopsia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 40:5, 1977

287. Young WB, Heros DO, Ehrenberg BL, Hedges TR: Metamorphopsia and palinopsia: Association with periodic lateralizing epileptiform discharges in a patient with malignant astrocytoma. Arch Neurol 46:820, 1989

288. Swash M: Visual perseveration in temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 42:569, 1979

289. Blythe IM, Bromley JM, Ruddock KH et al: A study of systematic visual perseveration involving central mechanisms. Brain 106:661, 1986

290. Purvin VA: Visual disturbances secondary to clomiphene citrate. Arch Ophthalmol 113:482, 1995

291. Friedman DI, Hu EH, Sadun AA: Neuro-ophthalmic complications of interleukin 2 therapy. Arch Ophthalmol 109:1679, 1991

292. Hughes MS, Lessell S: Trazodone-induced palinopsia. Arch Ophthalmol 18:399, 1990

293. Marneros A, Korner J: Chronic palinopsia in schizophrenia. Psychopathology 26:236, 1993

294. Gates TJ, Stagno SJ, Gulledge AD: Palinopsia posing as psychotic depression. Br J Psychiatry 153:391, 1988

295. Jacome DE: Palinopsia and bitemporal visual extinction on fixation. Ann Ophthalmol 17:251, 1985

296. Lepore FE: Spontaneous visual phenomena with visual loss: 104 patients with lesions of retinal and neural afferent pathways. Neurology 40:444, 1990

297. Kölmel HW: Complex visual hallucinations in the hemianopic field. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 48:29, 1985

298. Schultz G, Melzack R: The Charles Bonnet syndrome: ‘Phantom visual images.’ Perception 20:809, 1991

299. Teunisse RJ, Cruysberg JRM, Verbeek A, Zitman FG: The Charles Bonnet syndrome: A large prospective study in the Netherlands. Br J Psychiatry 166:254, 1995

300. Kölmel HW: Coloured patterns in hemianopic fields. Brain 107:155, 1984

301. Plant GT: A centrally generated coloured phosphene. Clin Vis Sci 1:161, 1986

302. Anderson SW, Rizzo M: Hallucinations following occipital lobe damage: The pathological activation of visual representations. J Clin Exp Neurol 16:651, 1994

303. Weinberger LM, Grant FC: Visual hallucinations and their neuro-optical correlates. Arch Ophthalmol 23:166, 1940

304. Cogan DG: Visual hallucinations as release phenomenon. Albrecht von Graefe's Arch Klin Exp Ophthalmol 188:139, 1973

305. Lance JW: Simple formed hallucinations confined to the area of a specific visual field defect. Brain 99:719, 1976

306. Siatkowski RM, Zimmer B, Rosenberg PR: The Charles Bonnet syndrome: Visual perceptive dysfunction in sensory deprivation. J Clin Neuro Ophthalmol 10:215, 1990

307. Heron W: The pathology of boredom. Sci Am 196:52, 1957

308. Adair DK, Keshaven MS: The Charles Bonnet syndrome and grief reaction. Am J Psychiatry 145:895, 1988

309. Cole MG: Charles Bonnet hallucinations: A case series. Can J Psychiatry 37:267, 1992

310. Schultz G, Melzack R: Visual hallucinations and mental state: A study of 14 Charles Bonnet syndrome hallucinators. J Nerv Ment Dis 181:639, 1993

311. Bhatia MS, Khastgir U, Malik SC: Charles Bonnet syndrome. Br J Psychiatry 161:409, 1992

312. Schedlack KJ, McDonald WM, Laskowitz DT, Krishnan KRR: Geniculocalcarine hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging associated with visual hallucinations in the elderly. Psychiatry Res 54:283, 1994

313. Fisman M: Musical hallucinations: Report of two unusual cases. Can J Psychiatry 36:609, 1991

314. Penfield W, Perot P: The brain's record of auditory and visual experience. Brain 86:595, 1963

315. Foerster O: The cerebral cortex in man. Lancet 2:309, 1931

316. Rousseau M, Debrock D, Cabaret M, Steinling M: Visual hallucinations with written words in a case of left parietotemporal lesion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57:1268, 1994

317. Cohen L, Verstichel P, Pierrot-Deseilligny C: Hallucinatory vision of a familiar face following right temporal damage. Neurology 42:2052, 1992

318. Panayiotopoulos CP: Elementary visual hallucinations in migraine and epilepsy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57:1371, 1994

319. Lance JW, Smee RI: Partial seizures with visual disturbance treated by radiotherapy of cavernous hemangioma. Ann Neurol 26:782, 1989

320. Gastaut H: A new type of epilepsy: Benign partial epilepsy of childhood with occipital spike-waves. Clin Electroencephalogr 13:13, 1982

321. Lance JW, Anthony M: Some clinical aspects of migraine: A prospective survey of 500 patients. Arch Neurol 15:356, 1966

322. Plant GT: The fortification spectra of migraine. Br Med J 293:1613, 1986

323. Richards W: The fortification illusions of migraine. Sci Am 224:89, 1971

324. Grüsser O-J: Migraine phosphenes and the retino-cortical magnification factor. Vision Res 35:1125, 1995

325. Fox PT, Mintun MA, Raichle ME et al: Mapping human visual cortex with positron emission tomography. Nature 323:806, 1986

326. Sereno MI, Dale AM, Reppas JB et al: Borders of multiple visual areas revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science 268:889, 1995

327. Ungerleider LG: Functional brain imaging studies of cortical mechanisms for memory. Science 270:769, 1995

328. Miezin FM, Fox PT, Raichle ME, Allman JM: Localized responses to low contrast moving random dot patterns in human visual cortex monitored with positron emission tomography (abstr). Soc Neurosci 13:631, 1987

329. Zeki S, Watson JDG, Lueck CJ et al: A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. J Neurosci 11:641, 1991

330. Watson JDG, Myers R, Frackowiak RSJ et al: Area V5 of the human brain: Evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex 3:79, 1993

331. Dupont P, Orban GA, de Bruyn B et al: Many areas in the human brain respond to visual motion. J Neurophysiol 72:1420, 1994

332. de Jong BM, Shipp S, Skidmore B et al: The cerebral activity related to the visual perception of forward motion in depth. Brain 117:1039, 1994

333. Tootell RBH, Reppas JB, Kwong KK et al: Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 15:3215, 1995

334. McCarthy G, Spicer M, Adrignolo A et al: Brain activation associated with visual motion studied by functional magnetic resonance imaging in humans. Human Brain Mapping 2:234, 1995

335. Tootell RBH, Reppas JB, Dale AM et al: Visual motion after-effect in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature 375:139, 1995

336. Probst T, Plendl H, Paulus W et al: Identification of the visual motion area (area V5) in the human brain by dipole source analysis. Exp Brain Res 93:345, 1993

337. Beckers G, Zeki S: The consequences of inactivating areas V1 and V5 in visual motion perception. Brain 118:49, 1995

338. Beckers G, Hömberg V: Cerebral visual motion blindness: transitory akinetopsia induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation of human area V5. Pro R Soc Lond [Biol] 249:173, 1992

339. Hotson J, Braun D, Herzberg W, Boman D: Transcranial magnetic stimulation of extrastriate cortex degrades human motion direction discrimination. Vision Res 34:2115, 1994

340. Tootell RBH, Hamilton SL, Silverman MS: Topography of cytochrome oxidase activity in owl monkey cortex. J Neurosci 5:2786, 1985

341. Tootell RBH, Taylor JB: Anatomical evidence for MT and additional cortical visual areas in humans. Cereb Cortex 5:39, 1995

342. DeYoe EA, Hockfield S, Garren H, van Essen DC: Antibody labelling of functional subdivisions in visual cortex: CAT-301 immunoreactivity in striate and extrastriate cortex of the macaque monkey. Vis Neurosci 5:67, 1990

343. Clarke S, Miklossy J: Occipital cortex in man: Organization of callosal connections, related myelo- and cytoarchitecture, and putative boundaries of functional visual areas. J Comp Neurol 298:188, 1990

344. Clarke S: Modular organization of human extrastriate visual cortex: Evidence from cytochrome oxidase pattern in normal and macular degeneration cases. Eur J Neurosci 6:725, 1994

345. Lueck CJ, Zeki S, Friston KJ et al: The colour center in the cerebral cortex of man. Nature 340:386, 1989

346. Gulyás B, Heywood CA, Popplewell DA et al: Visual form discrimination from color or motion cues: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91:9965, 1994

347. Gulyás B, Roland PE: Cortical fields participating in form and colour discrimination in the human brain. Neuroreport 2:585, 1991

348. Martin A, Haxby JV, Lalonde FM et al: Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science 270:102, 1995

349. Allison T, Begleiter A, McCarthy G et al: Electrophysiological studies of color processing in human visual cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 88:343, 1993

350. Sergent J, Ohta S, MacDonald B: Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study. Brain 115:15, 1992

351. Grady CL, Maisog JM, Horwitz B et al: Age-related changes in cortical blood flow activation during visual processing of faces and location. J Neurosci 14:1450, 1994

352. Haxby JV, Ungerleider LG, Horwitz B et al: Face encoding and recognition in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:922, 1996

353. Haxby JV, Horwitz B, Ungerleider LG et al: The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations. J Neurosci 14:6336, 1994

354. Kapur N, Friston KJ, Young A et al: Activation of human hippocampal formation during memory for faces: a PET study. Cortex 31:99, 1995

355. Puce A, Allison T, Gore JC, McCarthy G: Face-sensitive regions in human extrastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. J Neurophysiol 74:1192, 1995

356. McIntosh AR, Grady CL, Ungerleider LG et al: Network analysis of cortical visual pathways mapped with PET. J Neurosci 14:655, 1994

357. Dissociation of pathways for object and spatial vision: a PET study in humans. Neuroreport 6:1865, 1995

358. Corbetta M, Shulman GL, Miezin FM, Petersen SE: Superior parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and visual feature conjunction. Science 270:802, 1995

359. Petersen SE, Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Shulman GL: PET studies of parietal involvement in spatial attention: comparison of different task types. Can J Exp Psychol 48:319, 1994This work was supported by NIH grant No. PO NS 19632 and by a Medical Research Council of Canada Fellowship.

Back to Top